128 Kindergarten Research Topics & Essay Examples
đ kindergarten research papers examples, đ simple research topics about kindergarten, đ good kindergarten essay topics to write about, đ best kindergarten essay titles, â kindergarten research questions.
- Early Childhood Education: Studies Review Studies on early childhood education have been conducted in various countries. This paper reviews three studies that were conducted in Germany, Netherlands and the USA.
- Bilingual Education and Preschoolers' Development There are several theories in child development that directly or indirectly discuss the implications of bilingual education and show the importance of bilingual education.
- Engaging the Learner in the Early Years of Schooling The following project report is preparation of education plans for five- year-old Indonesian children who are now growing up in the alien lands of Sydney in Australia.
- Curriculum for Kindergarten Pupils The article presents a mini fitness lesson suitable for kindergarten children, as well as a plan for an interactive family event.
- Developmentally Appropriate Activities for Toddlers The study sample includes a book aimed at developing toddlersâ cognitive skills and gross motor behavior, a go-get interactive game, and a video on learning colors.
- Sloth Sky Preschool: Curriculum Context Sloth Sky Preschool offers center-based care for preschool/kindergarten children as a part of a school. Based on the location, the school is near Penguin Ocean Primary School.
- Special Education for Children of Kindergarten and Third-Grade Levels Special education is designed to address the needs of students with specific disabilities in a very individual manner.
- Literacy Development for Preschoolers The article focuses on the importance of early literacy development in preschoolers and methods that will help teachers in this.
- An Ethical Dilemma With a Child in a Preschool Classroom An ethical dilemma with a child in a preschool classroom stemming from a situation when a child does not want to comply with a particular EC institutionâs standard schedule.
- Influence of Teachersâ Attitudes on Student Behavior and Achievement This article is about a study that aims to determine how teacher attitudes affect student behavior and performance in grades kindergarten through fifth.
- How Teacher Attitudes Affect Students' Behavior and Performance in Grades The aim of conducting this study is to determine how teacher attitudes affect student behavior and performance in grades Kindergarten through fifth.
- The Aspects of Purposeful Preschool Program The article argues class management is one of the key components of teaching practice as it can ensure the proper development and academic achievement of preschool children.
- Preschool Teacher Practice Rotation Experience Preschool experience plays a pivotal role in a personâs development, as it lays the foundation for their character
- Using Data in Preschools: Data Collection Data drives every aspect of teaching, especially when there is a need to teach young children. Teachers need to see where the kids are thriving and the gaps that need to be filled.
- Preschooler Education Overview The article "Preschoolers (3-5 years of age)" describes preschoolers' development milestones aged between 3 to 5 and suggests some positive parenting tips.
- "Teaching Strategies for Preschool Educators" Analysis The article focuses on the main teaching strategies used in preschool education. The first strategy is the promotion of early literacy.
- Preschoolers Education and Activities for Children The activities for children should be appropriate for everyone within the class. Preschoolers must be encouraged to play and learn.
- Language Acquisition in Toddlers and Infants Language acquisition in toddlers starts from their communication with the environment. A friendly environment and proper materials allow for simplifying the language learning process
- Importance of the Assessment in Kindergarten and Its Types Assessment in kindergarten is essential in building future generations, building the pupils' strength and giving them the confidence is the joy of a teacher.
- âInvestigating Pathwaysâ: Toddler Learning Setups âInvestigating Pathwaysâ video examines two toddler learning setups: the blue room and the green room, each using different investigation concepts.
- Infant and Toddler Development and Programming The set of materials presented in this essay provides a beneficial learning process for babies and toddlers because their usage is organized around educational principles of belonging.
- Examination of Books and Interactive Content for Numbers Learning This paper identifies 12 textbooks that teach pre-kindergarten, kindergarten, and second-grade children to understand numbers, counting, and sets.
- Integrated Co-Teaching and Impact on Learning Integrated co-teaching (ICT) is an emerging method of teaching students with learning disabilities. This system is implemented in different levels of educational institutions.
- Number Sense and Learning Centers in Preschool To improve Number Sense in preschool, the educator will have to adopt new learning strategies. The expectations of the learning outcomes are shaped by curriculum.
- The Quality of Learning Programs in Preschool Establishments The paper research and summarizes the article "Quality 101: Identifying the core components of a high-quality early childhood program" about high-quality child care programs.
- Visiting a Preschool Class as Reflective Practice This report provides a detailed reflection of the activities, environment, and knowledge obtained during a visit to a preschool class in one of the local public schools.
- Strive Preschool: Using Information Systems in a Preschool The school should start by establishing the necessary roles and activities and using IT budget to buy, maintain, and update software and hardware.
- Special Education Curriculum Issues and Their Solutions in the USA There are many potential improvements that can transform the special education programs across the US to become more efficient, inclusive, and appropriate for the stakeholders.
- Lesson Plan for Pre-Kindergarteners Learning activities have to be divided into relevant categories to help the educator pick a small number of developmental milestones and focus on individual elements of learning.
- A Fire Safety Class Held in a Preschool The paper states that the case illustrates a fire safety class held in a preschool. Children were taught the basics of emergency case information.
- Froebelâs Kindergarten Philosophy Friedrich Froebel is a renowned philosopher whose influence on childhood education is evident in the contemporary world.
- Planning and Teaching in a Preschool Setting The teacher's job is to plan learning activities that begin with the development of learning objectives and continue through implementation and evaluation in a preschool setting.
- Physical Development in Florida Voluntary Prekindergarten Education Program The observed Pre-K program had an emphasis on physical development, which incorporated elements of math, reading, music, and arts.
- Lesson Plans for Preschool and Second Grade During the math lesson, it is necessary to observe whether children show increased use of part-whole skills, and division of things into smaller objects.
- Preschool Education in China and Japan Preschool education in China serves several purposes, from child care to educational preparation. Meanwhile, the Japanese preschool system is more directed toward socialization.
- Education System: Building a Class The learning process aims to develop professional skills and develop tolerance and create a picture of the world.
- Importance of Research-Based Curriculum The Fireflies is both a developmentally appropriate and engaging curriculum that allows the teacher to promote learning through play and help children.
- Literacy in Elementary Schools After COVID-19 Pandemic The level of literacy the students of kindergartens and first three grades of elementary schools possess is lower than it was before the COVID-19 pandemic.
- The Preschool Program's Design Analysis This paper aims to design a preschool daycare program for children aged three to five and discuss the activities to enhance their social, emotional, physical, and cognitive skills.
- Early Childhood Education in India Today's preschool education system in India is designed so that parents can rest assured of their children who have been trained in such groups.
- Social-Emotional Skills Program for Preschoolers During the early 21st century, it was reported that the ability to regulate behaviors and emotions could be seen as the fundamental skills showcasing a childâs school readiness.
- Comparison of the Two Early Childhood Educational Institutions in Hong Kong This paper examines the educational and administrative management aspects of two kindergartens in Hong Kong: HKYWCA Athena and The Salvation Army Shui Chuen O Kindergarten.
- Research in the Field of Childhood Literacy This paper contains an annotated bibliography of the two articles devoted to the topics of childhood literacy and education.
- Social-Emotional Skill Program to Enhance Learning in Preschoolers This research studies the relationship between three variables, the learners' socio-emotional skills and their academic and behavioral outcomes in pre-schoolers.
- Me and Others: Observation of Preschool Children This paper analyzes the video âMe and othersâ, which shows an experiment on suggestibility performed with preschool children (5 years old).
- âThe Kindergarten Programâ: Visible Learning in Early Childhood Education Visible learning is particularly significant since children learn from experience and should be exposed to real-life situations.
- Interventions and Letter & Sound Recognition Students who experience difficulties in recognizing letters and sounds in words and reading face challenges in writing and isolation of alphabetical sounds.
- Dyslexia in Young Children: Developmental Language Disorder ADHD, autism spectrum disorder, depression, anorexia, Tourette's, and bipolar are among those conditions that have various symptoms.
- Early Childhood Education Programs Comparison Relying on the two videos on Early Childhood Education Programs, this paper compares various programs that are critical for the proper development of children.
- Preschool Activity: Introducing Multicultural Awareness The world is a place of many cultures and nations, which is why it is essential to introduce multicultural awareness to children.
- The Lingua Globe Educational Toy The Lingua Globe toy represents a globe that features multiple buttons corresponding to different countries. The toy addresses primarily the linguistic aspect of development.
- Early Childhood Education: Pedagogical Skills Understanding each child as a unique individual with their own psychological characteristics and structure of thinking seems to be a necessary competence of any children's teacher.
- Positive Behaviour Support Framework in Preschool The School-Wide Positive Behaviour Support framework promotes acceptable behavior expectations for children in schools to facilitate a safe learning culture.
- The Overall Expectations in the Kindergarten Program The documentation The Overall Expectations in the Kindergarten Program supports the idea that the cup-stacking activity addresses the two overall expectation frames.
- Mutual Storytelling in the Baby's Edcuation Communicating with your child is an essential aspect of learning because from a very early age, children try to copy the behavior of adults.
- The Montessori Method and Its Benefits The Montessori Method of education is an influential teaching method to ensure that students are equipped with the best practical skills in the learning process.
- Expanding Existing Programs and Services in Preschool The expansion of the existing program and service in preschool is a good project which needs to be implemented in the West Bronx neighborhood.
- DIBELS: The Cognitive Foundations of Reading and Its Acquisition DIBELS is a literacy assessment program that helps identify and offer assistance to lagging students. The key tests in the DIBELS program are the LNF, ISF, PSF, and NWF tests.
- Early Childhood Education Aspects Early childhood is considered the most crucial time of child development because it is a period of fast physical and mental development.
- Family Partnerships to Help Needy Preschooler Students Family and parent partnerships are essential to ensuring that students build strong relationships that can help in boosting their success.
- Educational Assistant: Roles and Responsibilities Educational assistants monitor and record learners' needs, strengths, behavior, and achievements during their time in school and report crucial incidents.
- Should Pre-K Be Made Available to All Children? Preparing children for elementary school can benefit them because preschool improves their cognitive abilities and gets them used to a regimented schedule.
- A Description of the Student Observation The paper includes a description of the setting, including one's first impressions, suitability for children, and the school environment and atmosphere.
- English Language Learners and Kindergarten Entry Age: Achievement and Social-emotional Effects
- Preschool and Kindergarten Best Practice Curriculum
- Language Acquisition and Expected Developmental Differences for Children of Preschool, Kindergarten, and Primary Ages
- The Lesson Plan Created for Kindergarten Students Based on Next Generation
- The Need for Universal All Day Pre Kindergarten
- Intervention and Theoretical Paper Kindergarten Age
- Full-Day vs Half-Day Kindergarten: Which Is Better? This research used an experimental method to point out the cause and effect of having children enrolled in a full-day or half-day kindergarten program.
- The Literacy Improvement Program for Pre Kindergarten
- Teacher Quality and Learning Outcomes in Kindergarten
- Kindergarten Childrenâs Attachment Security, Inhibitory Control, and the Internalization of Rules of Conduct
- Getting Mom Ready for Kindergarten
- Developmental Differences That Can Be Expected in Language Acquisition for Preschool, Kindergarten and Primary Aged Children
- The Challenges Facing Kindergarten Teachers for Deal With Children Whom English Is an Additional Language
- Teaching English Language in Saudi Arabia Kindergarten
- This research will primarily explore the significance of integrating English in Kindergarten, which have started emerging in Saudi Arabia, in both private and public schools.
- Team Work, Connection and Cooperation Among Two Institutions â School and Kindergarten
- Shared Book Reading Promotes Not Only Language Development, but Also Grapheme Awareness in German Kindergarten Children
- Factors Affecting the School Readiness of Public Kindergarten Pupils in Lanao Del Sur IâB
- High-Quality Pre Kindergarten Classroom Look Like
- The Kindergarten Attendance Allowance in Hungary
- Using Kindergarten Number Sense To Predict Calculation Fluency in Second Grade
- The Full Day Kindergarten Program
- Effects of Kindergarten Activities on Cognitive Development This paper explores how teachers and parents can enhance the development of cognitive skills of kindergarteners through engagement with nuts and bolts.
- Kindergarten Enrollment and the Intergenerational Transmission of Education
- Procedures for Sampling Texts and Tasks in Kindergarten Through Eighth Grade
- Relationship Between Kindergarten Organizational Climate and Teacher Burnout
- Parents and Kindergarten Decisions To Be Made
- Kindergarten and Education Expansion Programme Analysis
- Kindergarten for All: Long-run Effects of a Universal Intervention
- Teacher Interview and Observation of a Kindergarten Math
- Learning Activity That Has Been Designed for the Kindergarten
- Investigation Into Developed Sabis Curriculum in Kindergarten
- Teaching Young Children and Becoming a Kindergarten Teacher
- Childâs Development Analysis and Potential Interventions In the case of the child described in the paper, there was a normal transition â she started studying in kindergarten and a non-normal one â she experienced her parents’ divorce.
- Residential Mobility Across Early Childhood and Childrenâs Kindergarten Readiness
- Young Childrenâs Developmental Ecologies and Kindergarten Readiness
- Kindergarten and Child Primary Education
- Technology and the Classroom Starting With Kindergarten
- Kindergarten Through Sixth Grade Writing Effectiveness Instruction
- Children and Home Reading Before Kindergarten
- Early Kindergarten, Maternal Labor Supply, and Childrenâs Outcomes: Evidence From Italy
- Physical and Technological Resources in Kindergarten
- Full-Day Kindergarten Versus Half Day Kindergarten Reading Achievement
- Efficiency and Fair Access in Kindergarten Allocation Policy Design
- Student Engagement and Student Motivation in a Reading Classroom for the Kindergarten Level
- Why Is Partnership Between Parents and Kindergarten Teachers Necessary?
- What Is the Relationship Between Kindergarten and Primary School Teacher?
- How Does the Developed Sabis Curriculum Affect Children in Kindergarten?
- Why Should Foreign Language Teaching Begin in Kindergarten?
- How Non-native Speaking Children Learn the Chinese Language in Kindergarten
- Does Delaying Kindergarten Entrance Give Children a Head Start?
- What Is the Difference Between Elementary School and Kindergarten?
- Can Universal Pre-kindergarten Programs Improve Population Health and Longevity?
- What Are Free Kindergarten Educational Programs?
- Sharing Responsibility: What They Didnât Teach You in Kindergarten?
- Why Do Kindergarten Teachers Need To Have at Least a Basic Degree?
- What Is the Relationship of Environmental Instability and the Behavior of Children in the Classroom in Kindergarten?
- What Do Kindergarten Teachers and University Professors Have in Common?
- Do Kindergarten Students Need Computer Science Classes?
- Why Does the Montana School System Require Sex Education for Kindergarten?
- How Do Preschool Programs Improve Literacy and Achievement?
- What Are the Duties of Kindergarten Teachers?
- How and Why Does Age at Kindergarten Entry Matter?
- What Is the Importance of Full-Day Kindergarten?
- How Do Kindergarten Prep Skills Contribute to Academic Achievement?

Cite this page
Select style
- Chicago (A-D)
- Chicago (N-B)
ChalkyPapers. (2023, October 5). 128 Kindergarten Research Topics & Essay Examples. Retrieved from https://chalkypapers.com/topics/kindergarten-research-topics/
ChalkyPapers. (2023, October 5). 128 Kindergarten Research Topics & Essay Examples. https://chalkypapers.com/topics/kindergarten-research-topics/
"128 Kindergarten Research Topics & Essay Examples." ChalkyPapers , 5 Oct. 2023, chalkypapers.com/topics/kindergarten-research-topics/.
ChalkyPapers . (2023) '128 Kindergarten Research Topics & Essay Examples'. 5 October.
ChalkyPapers . 2023. "128 Kindergarten Research Topics & Essay Examples." October 5, 2023. https://chalkypapers.com/topics/kindergarten-research-topics/.
1. ChalkyPapers . "128 Kindergarten Research Topics & Essay Examples." October 5, 2023. https://chalkypapers.com/topics/kindergarten-research-topics/.
Bibliography
ChalkyPapers . "128 Kindergarten Research Topics & Essay Examples." October 5, 2023. https://chalkypapers.com/topics/kindergarten-research-topics/.
- Sex Education
- School Bullying
- Online Education
- Early Childhood Education
- Childhood Education
- Adult Learning

5 Research-based Practices for Kindergarten and First Grade
Here's what every kindergarten and first grade teacher should be doing..
Posted June 1, 2017
[Note: In this guest post, two renowned former kindergarten teachers who wrote about the extraordinary literacy successes of children in their classrooms (Feldgus & Cardonick, 1999) show how todayâs cutting -edge research in cognitive psychology and neuroscience supports the best practices they and their colleagues have advocated and honed for three decades. What they discovered intuitively and through reflection and collaboration is now cutting-edge best practice in 21st century kindergartens.]
5 Powerful Research-Based Techniques for Exemplary Kindergartens Today
By Eileen Feldgus PhD and Isabell Cardonick MEd
The recent high-profile spotlight on a landmark study in Developmental Psychology has drawn attention to research by Canadian cognitive psychologists Gene Ouellette and Monique SĂ©nĂ©chal (2017). In some respects Ouellette and SĂ©nĂ©chal discovered what we, Eileen and Isabell, have advocated for decades: a powerful connection to improved end-of-first-grade reading scores through the use of early writing and invented spelling. The studyâs title, âInvented Spelling in Kindergarten as a Predictor of Reading and Spelling in Grade 1: A New Pathway to Literacy, or Just the Same Road, Less Known?â(Ouellette and SĂ©nĂ©chal, 2017), reflects what exemplary kindergarten teachers have known about the powerful writing/reading connection for yearsâkid writing is a pathway to reading success. But this work is still not well known or universally practiced. This less-known pathway is the one we traveled. Starting out as passionate kindergarten teachers in the 1960âs and 70âs, we were ardent about creating classrooms that worked for children. We devoured the research of that era and became life-long learners throughout our careers. Early on we discovered better outcomes for children as we focused more on writing, encouraged invented spelling, developed innovative strategies for teaching phonics and eliminated boring worksheets. Today in 2017 five best-practice techniques we discovered in our practice are now wholly supported by research and recommended for todayâs kindergartens and first grades.
1. Use Invented Spelling (Ouellette & SĂ©nĂ©chal, 2017). We found invented spelling to be joyful, motivational for our students, and wonderful in terms of providing opportunities for scaffolding and systematically teaching almost all important aspects of the kindergarten literacy curriculum including phonics, phonemic awareness, knowledge of the alphabet, writing conventions, and vocabulary development. But perhaps the most amazing discovery throughout our journey was that kids had remarkable capacities to make meaning if we supported them in the process and allowed their creative juices to flow. Early on we learned as we had read in Don Gravesâ research (1983), that kids write best when we step back and allow them to choose their own topics and give them ownership and autonomy. We called our teaching model, âKid Writingâ (1999). Our model fit perfectly with a growing model now called Guided Reading for differentiated reading instruction (Fountas & Pinnell, 2012) and we put in a whole layer underpinning Lucy Calkins work (2003) by showing teachers exactly how to get started and how to move forward with writing workshop and formative assessment. Here are a few samples that illustrate kidsâ capacity to grow and flourish as writers in kindergarten:

When I was born my mommy and daddy used to pay a lot of attention to me. But now they donât pay a lot of attention to me. They pay a lot to Conner.

2. Abandon teaching âLetter of the Week (Reutzel, 1992, 2015). Teaching one letter per week was standard practice in kindergarten when we began teaching. We tried our best to jazz up our teaching of the alphabetic principle because we knew it was essential to breaking the code and reading. Our students sang for the letter, danced for the letter, cooked for the letter, and cut and pasted for the letter. We took elaborate measures to teach the alphabet and sounds because we knew it was important. One fond memory was our âPâ Party:â we served foods beginning with Pâpizza, pretzels, popcorn, pepperoni, and the like. But with letter of the week the pace was too slow, and as far back as 1992 researchers were noticing the same problem and cautioning teachers to âbreak the letter-a-week tradition.â (Reutzel, 1992)
So in our classrooms we began to use childrenâs names on the first day of kindergartenâfrom Albert to Zoieâand learned to focus on all the sounds and letters from the very beginning. In contrast to when we were using letter of the week our students mastered letters and sounds far sooner.
Today, as reported by Reutzel (2015), âresearch has identified six evidence- based alphabet letter learning orders through which young children may acquire knowledge of alphabet letter names and sounds (Justice, Pence, Bowles, & Wiggins, 2006 ).â And guess what? âThe first learning order is called the own-name effect.â (Reutzel, 2015) We got it right before the research proved it!
3. Use Invented Spelling and a Developmental Writing Scale to monitor progress (Gentry, 2006, 2000). Even before we published the first book on Kid Writing, we were collaborating with Richard Gentry on how to use a developmental spelling/writing assessment along with a developmental rubric to show how young childrenâs progression through five phases of developmental spelling revealedâamong other thingsâthe individual childâs understanding of phonics and his or her invented spellings as evidence of what the child knew or did not know. We found this work to be much more powerful for targeting instruction and monitoring kindergartnersâ progress than traditional spelling tests or even measures of phonemic awareness and alphabet knowledge. Progress monitoring by phase observation is now supported by empirical research! (Ouellette & SĂ©nĂ©chal, 2017)
When we started out as neophyte teachers, kids were simply memorizing words that we gave them on a list. We learned to scaffold what they were using in their invented spelling and to show them how English spelling works. Our teaching went from giving lists of what to spell to showing kids how to spell and invented spelling was our vehicle! Without getting into the particulars of an analysis, look at the following samples that show one kindergarten childâs remarkable progress from fall to spring of her kindergarten year.

September sample: It was a sunny day.
June sample: Tuesday my tooth was wiggling. When it was in my mouth, it bled. When it fell out, it stopped bleeding. My mom gently pulled it out with a paper towel and I was happy that it fell out.
4. Let go of worksheets! (Palmer & Invernizzi, 2015). We found that teaching and learning in our classrooms improved when we abandoned worksheets. Remember those nonsensical work sheets where children were to write the letter that the word for each picture would begin with? When we first began teaching we remember students who squirmed with sit-at-the-desk busy worksheets and struggled over the Y is for Yak worksheet wondering why Y was the match for the first sound in âgoatâ which is the picture they saw on the worksheet.
5. Teach children to stretch though a word with a moving target. (Feldgus, Cardonick, & Gentry, 2017) Research by Ouelette, validated our Stretching Through a Word with a Moving Target teaching methodology. Their research, âconfirmed that facilitating invented spelling within a Vygotskian teaching approach can bring about benefits in learning to read and spell, and these benefits go beyond the expansion of alphabetic knowledge and phonological awareness.

Our stretching through technique, for example, helped kids move from l for lady in Phase 2 to lad in Phase 3 to ladee in syllable chunks in Phase 4 on the way to conventional lady . The stretching through technique met kids where they were and supported them in moving to higher levels of spelling sophistication from phase to phase.
Keep the FaithâKeep the PassionâKeep Your Kids Writing
One thing that hasnât changed over the years is our passion for literacy-learning classrooms for beginners. Today as staff developers and authors, we continue to encounter kindergarten teachers all over America and beyond who share our passion, devotion to children, and vision for joyful, play-based, academic kindergarten and first grade classrooms. We believe implementing these five research-based strategies surrounding kid writing will be transformational in America. It is the answer to reversing the decades-old trend of flat-lined first grade reading scores!
For details on these five strategies and creating joyful kid-writing classrooms that work, check out our comprehensive guide for kindergarten and grade 1 teachers: Kid Writing in the 21st Century: A Systematic Approach to Phonics, Spelling and Writing Workshop (Hameray, 2017).
Link to Kid Writing to learn more.
Dr. J. Richard Gentry is the author of Raising Confident Readers, How to Teach Your Child to Read and WriteâFrom Baby to Age 7 . Follow him on Facebook , Twitter , and LinkedIn and find out more information about his work on his website .
Calkins, L. M. (2003). The Nuts and Bolts of Teaching Writing. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Feldgus, E., Cardonick, I. & Gentry, J. R. (2017). Kid Writing in the 21st Century . Las Angeles, CA: Hameray Publishing Group.
Fountas, I. and Pinnell, G.S.(2013). The Reading Teacher. 66 (4) 268-284.
Gentry, R. (2006). Breaking the code: The new science of beginning reading and writing . Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Gentry, R. (2000). A retrospective on invented spelling and a look forward. The Reading Teacher . 54 (3) 318-332.
Graves, D. H. (1983). Writing: Teachers and Children at Work . Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Justice , L.M. , Pence , K. , Bowles , R.B. , & Wiggins , A. ( 2006 ). An investigation of four hypotheses concerning the order by which 4- year- old children learn the alphabet letters. Early Childhood Research Quarterly , 21 ( 3), 374 â 389 .
Ouelette, G. & SĂ©nĂ©chal, M. (2017). Invented spelling in kindergarten as a predictor of reading and spelling in grade 1: A new Pathway to literacy, or just the same road, less known? Developmental Psychology . 53 (1) 77â 88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000179
Palmer, J. & Invernizzi, M. (2015). No More Phonics and Spelling Worksheets . Portsmouth NH: Heinemann.
Reutzel, D. R. (2015). Early literacy research: Findings primary-grade teachers will want to know. The Reading Teacher . 69, (1), 14â24. DOI: 10.1002/trtr.1387 © 2015 International Literacy Association
Reutzel , D.R. ( 1992 ). Breaking the letter- a- week tradition: Conveying the alphabetic principle to young children. Childhood Education , 69 ( 1 ), 20 â 23 .

J. Richard Gentry, Ph.D. , is an expert on childhood literacy, reading, and spelling. He is the author of Raising Confident Readers: H ow to Teach Your Child to Read and WriteâBaby to Age 7 .
- Find a Therapist
- Find a Treatment Center
- Find a Psychiatrist
- Find a Support Group
- Find Teletherapy
- United States
- Brooklyn, NY
- Chicago, IL
- Houston, TX
- Los Angeles, CA
- New York, NY
- Portland, OR
- San Diego, CA
- San Francisco, CA
- Seattle, WA
- Washington, DC
- Asperger's
- Bipolar Disorder
- Chronic Pain
- Eating Disorders
- Passive Aggression
- Personality
- Goal Setting
- Positive Psychology
- Stopping Smoking
- Low Sexual Desire
- Relationships
- Child Development
- Therapy Center NEW
- Diagnosis Dictionary
- Types of Therapy

The people around us have a stronger influence on our decisions and actions than we realize. Hereâs what research reveals about our networksâ gravitational force.
- Coronavirus Disease 2019
- Affective Forecasting
- Neuroscience
kindergarten research writing templates
All Formats
Resource types, all resource types, kindergarten research writing templates.
- Rating Count
- Price (Ascending)
- Price (Descending)
- Most Recent

Research Report Writing Unit - Kindergarten , 1st Grade Research Study Template

Animal Reports - Research Template & Writing Papers-UPDATED

Animal Research BUNDLE (K-1)

Almost Gone...An Endangered Animal Research and Writing Unit

ELEPHANT ACTIVITIES NONFICTION AND FICTION for Kindergarten and First Grade

OCEAN ANIMALS Research Templates Writing Flap Books! ESL Science Activities

ARCTIC POLAR Non Fiction Animal Differeniated Research Posters Writing Templates

ARCTIC ANIMALS UNIT FOR KINDERGARTEN | THE BUNDLE

Animal Research for First Grade: Research Templates & Guides for Primary Writing

OCEAN ANIMALS Non Fiction Differeniated Research Posters Writing Templates

Arctic Animals Unit for Kindergarten | Arctic Fox Activities

Research Paper Outline BUNDLE! With 3 Research Writing [ Template ] Essay Outlines

Arctic Animals Unit for Kindergarten | Reindeer or Caribou Activities

Research Paper Outline [ Template ] - Animal | Research Writing Essay Outline
- Easel Activity

All About Planets Writing Research Templates

PENGUINS Research Writing Flap Book with 30 Differentiated Writing Templates

Arctic Animals Unit for Kindergarten | Puffin Activities

Dinosaurs and Prehistoric Creatures Posters and Research Activities

FOREST ANIMALS Non Fiction Differentiated Research Posters Writing Templates

Polar Bear Research Writing Flap Book with 30 Differentiated Writing Templates

Arctic Animals Unit for Kindergarten | Polar Bears

Arctic Animals Unit for Kindergarten | Harp Seal Activities

Arctic Animals Unit for Kindergarten | Penguin Activities

Biography Research with PebbleGo

- We're hiring
- Help & FAQ
- Privacy policy
- Student privacy
- Terms of service
- Tell us what you think
- 2022 AERA in the News
- 2021 AERA In the News
- 2020 AERA In the News
- 2019 AERA In the News
- 2018 AERA In the News
- 2017 AERA In the News
- 2016 AERA In the News
- 2015 AERA In the News
- 2014 AERA In the News
- 2013 AERA In the News
- AERA Speaking Out on Major Issues
- 2022 AERA News Releases
- 2021 AERA News Releases
- 2020 AERA News Releases
- 2019 AERA News Releases
- 2018 AERA News Releases
- 2017 AERA News Releases
- 2016 AERA News Releases
- 2015 AERA News Releases
- 2014 AERA News Releases
- 2013 AERA News Releases
- 2012 AERA News Releases
- 2011 News Releases
- 2010 News Releases
- 2009 News Releases
- 2008 News Releases
- 2007 News Releases
- 2006 News Releases
- 2005 News Releases
- 2004 News Releases
- AERA Research Archive
- Trending Topic Research Files
- Communication Resources for Researchers
- AERA Highlights Archival Issues
- AERA Video Gallery

Share
Early Childhood Education Research Paper

View sample early childhood education research paper. Browse research paper examples for more inspiration. If you need a psychology research paper written according to all the academic standards, you can always turn to our experienced writers for help. This is how your paper can get an A! Feel free to contact our writing service for professional assistance. We offer high-quality assignments for reasonable rates.
This research paper begins with a glance in the rearview mirror to consider what different sources had to say in the first half of the twentieth century. In 1929 the National Society for the Study of Education (NSSE) published its annual yearbook on the topic of preschool and parental education, in which editors referred to a
Academic Writing, Editing, Proofreading, And Problem Solving Services
Get 10% off with fall23 discount code.
new and different conception of the educational significance of the first half-dozen years of life. This new conception of the significance of the preschool period has led to the development of several new educational activities, more especially to the development of nursery schools and of new organizations and methods for the better training of parents. (NSSE, 1929, p. iv)
From the 1920s to the 1950s preschool or nursery school education were considered by most educators, researchers, and parents to be the only legitimate manifestation of what was referred to as early childhood education. The 1929 yearbook refers to full-day child care programs as âday nurseries,â which were part of the parallel and very different world of child welfare, clearly distinct in purpose, orientation, and content from the early childhood education programs of nursery schools. When the 1939â1940 yearbook titled Intelligence: Its Nature and Nurture was published (NSSE, 1939), the primary focus was, again, on one kind of setting (half-day nursery school programs) and on one specific outcome (IQ scores). Titles of chapters in the 1939â1940 yearbook included the following:
- âA Longitudinal Study of the Effects of Nursery-School Training on Successive Intelligence-Test Ratings,â
- âThe Effect of Nursery-School Attendance Upon Mental Growth of Children,â
- âInfluence of the Nursery School on Mental Growth,â
- âThe Mental Development of Nursery School Children Compared With That of Non-Nursery-School Children,â
- âMentalGrowthasRelatedtoNurserySchoolAttendance,â
- âA Follow-Up Study of a Group of Nursery School Children,â and
- âSubsequent Growth of Children With and Without Nursery School Experience.â
Early childhood educationâas a discipline, a discourse of inquiry, and a body of educational practices and approachesâhas changed dramatically since those early chapters written by the leading scholars of the day.The current research paper does not attempt to be either âdefinitiveâ or âcomprehensive,â but it does attempt to provide an accurate reflection of the diversity of philosophies, theories, and practices into which the field of early childhood education has matured. We no longer identify one specific program as âtypicalâ or model, and the once-firm line between âchild welfareâ and âeducationalâ programs has all but disappeared. In this research paper we consider the wide range of settings in which young children participate from birth to approximately age 8 years. The paper is designed to provide an understanding of both the breadth and depth of early childhood education and, in so doing, to give the reader the opportunity to become acquainted with what we consider to be some of the major themes in this field. It is our intention that the reader will gain this level of understanding and will be able to use the paper to identify further sources of information and knowledge based on what we present here.
This research paper on early childhood education draws on Bronfenbrennerâs ecological model of child development in order to share our understandings of the most recent developments in early childhood education and research at the different nested systemic levels of analysis (Bronfenbrenner, 1979; Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998). At the center of these nested concentric levels is the microsystem, which is seen as the immediate and naturalistic ecological niche within which the child develops. Examples of microsystems are home environments, child care settings, and classrooms. The mesosystem is the network of relationships among the various microsystem environments. For example, the degree of continuity or conflict between the home environments and school environments is a mesosystem dynamic. Embedding both of these inner systems is what Bronfenbrenner refers to as the exosystem, which includes broader societal influences that impact on the meso- and microsystems. Exosystem factors include legislation and social policy, labor force participation rates, neighborhood characteristics, teacher training programs, and the like. The overarching macrosystem refers to the societal and cultural values, beliefs, and attitudes that shape and influence the creation of policies and programs that ultimately impact on the lives of young children and their families.
In the first section, titled âLearning and Teaching in Early Childhood Education: Art, Literacy, Music, and Play,â we focus on child growth and activity as it is observed within the early childhood microsystem environments in which young children play, learn, and develop. In this section we explore what is known about the abilities and interests that young children bring to early childhood settings, and how the early childhood environment can challenge, engage, and interact with their abilities and interests. Within and across the areas of art, play, literacy, and music this section explores the dynamic tension between more endogenous and maturational views of early education and development at one end of the continuum and an approach that advocates for more direct instruction at the other.
The second section, âDiversity in Early Childhood Education: Individual Exceptionality and Cultural Pluralism,â considers the importance and impact of diversity in early childhood education. Although much of the work in early childhood education in the early twentieth century represented an attempt to define a set of universal norms, our understanding today is that early childhood education includes a very wide continuum of children with diverse abilities and disabilities who come from an ever increasing range of cultural and linguistic backgrounds. Rather than attempting to reduce the field to a few narrow common denominators, we wish to reflect the diversity that enlivens this field and challenges practitioners, researchers, and policy analysts. In this section we maintain our focus on the microsystems of early childhood education but broaden our perspective to include the mesosystem relationships among the various microsystems, and we begin to explore the exosystem levels of the social, legal, and regulatory contexts of early childhood education.
In the third section, âPrograms and Quality in Early Childhood Education,â we attempt to integrate theory, research, and practice in early childhood education as we consider the impact of ECE programs on the children and families who participate in them. Specifically, we examine two of the most dominant forms of ECE programming: compensatory education programs for at-risk children and nonparental child care programs. The issue of quality relates directly to the notion of an ecology of early childhood care, education, and development. Within this complex ecology there are dynamic interactions between the endogenous, biological factors of individual children and the environmental influences of classrooms, curricula, and the social, economic, and demographic realities that frame the development and education of young children. This framework, in turn, is informed by societal values and beliefs that contribute to the very definition of âqualityâ in ECE programs. In this third section we present a model of program quality that draws on empirical data from the different systemic levels of early childhood education and child care programs, across the curricular areas discussed in the first section of this research paper and in recognition of the individual differences and cultural diversity that are presented in the second section.
Learning and Teaching in Early Childhood: Art, Literacy, Music, and Play
Many textbooks on early childhood education are organized by curriculum subject areas with discrete how-to chapters on teaching literacy, music, art, science, social studies, movement, and mathematics to young children. The lives and abilities of young children are not so easily compartmentalized. For this reason we take a different tack when considering what children learn and how children learn in their early childhood years. We consider the ways in which children comprehend and represent their own lived realities across subject areas and domains of development. We open this discussion with an exploration of the nature and significance of art in early childhood education and artistic development in children. This discussion of the representational and semiotic functions of childrenâs art is followed by a consideration of childrenâs representations of thoughts, scripts, and inner realities in their spontaneous play. We then turn to childrenâs early encounters with the world of literacy in which the written word is seen as yet another powerful representation of both thought and language. The section concludes with an exploration of the significance of music, yet one additional form of representation and meaning in the childâs world that develops throughout the early childhood years
Art and Aesthetics in Early Childhood Education
The artistic production of young children has been a source of delight and enjoyment for many parents and early childhood educators. It is difficult to resist the simplicity of early forms that depict with enthusiasm and energy the worlds of childhood. It is hard not to be seduced by the directness and expressive quality of images that in their apparent inaccuracies, approximations, and technical deficiencies capture the element of youthful innocence that is but a memory in the adult universe. The aesthetic appeal of young childrenâs pictorial imagery seems to be closely bound to the emotional response that they evoke that allows for a nostalgic journey back to oneâs early years. Fineberg (1997) highlighted the significance of these sentiments in Picassoâs fascination with the âvisual inventiveness of childrenâ by pointing to its connection with the artistâs âown extraordinary access to the memories and urges of childhood that most of us have buried beyond the reach of our adult consciousnessâ (p. 137).
The value of early childhood as a period of uncontaminated purity where one has the benefit of perceiving the world through what Vasily Kandinsky (1912) called âunaccustomed eyesâ and thus has the ability to attend to the âpure inner toneâ of the objects in the world has been strongly reflected in the work of many artists of the modern area and has created a powerful legacy. Jean Dubuffet, who âturned in the 1940s to childrenâs drawings as a means of cutting to the truth of the ordinary experienceâ (Franciscono, 1998, p. 116); Joan Miro, whose work associated with the phenomenon of primitivism in French art of the 1920s and 1930s; and Paul Klee are among the icons of modernism who elevated childrenâs art to an unprecedented height. Consider Picassoâs famous remark made to Sir Herbert Read in 1956: âIt took me years to learn to draw like . . . children.â This one sentence captures the respect and admiration for young childrenâs artistic production shared by the most influential artists of the modern era (quoted by Fineberg, 1997, p. 133).
The sentiment expressed by Klee (1957) that âthe more helpless they [children] are, the more instructive their examples and already at an early stage one has to save them from corruptionâ (translation by Hofmann, 1998, p. 13) echoes in the works of one of the most influential Western art educators of the twentieth century, Victor Lowenfeld. In his most acclaimed work Creative and Mental Growth, Lowenfeld and Brittain (1964) argued that if âchildren developed without any interference from the outside world no special stimulation for their creative work would be necessary. Every child would use his deeply rooted creative impulse without inhibitionâ (p. 20). Needless to say, these assertions have never been empirically tested; nor have any scientific grounds been provided to offer them validity. Furthermore, it is impossible to conceive of circumstances in which children would grow up in environments devoid of visual manifestations of culture.
Yet, contemporary early childhood art education has been greatly affected by what had earlier been referred to as âa myth of natural unfoldingâ (Kindler, 1996). This is the notion that artistic abilities are contained within the child at birth and that a nonintrusive social environment is what is needed to bring them to the surface. This sentiment often echoes in early childhood texts and is well exemplified by Morgan (1988), who stated that we should consider âchildrenâs symbolic activity to be sacrosanct in early years . . . and therefore teacher intervention is inappropriateâ (p. 37). The nonintervention approach relies on the assumption that children are happy explorers who have an innate ability to satisfy their creative desires.
Art educators and researchers in day care, preschool, and primary classrooms have frequently witnessed childrenâs frustrating struggles with producing art rather than the happy explorations of young children trying to negotiate solutions to pictorial problems. Regretfully, many children emerge from these struggles with a loss of confidence that eventually leads to the abandonment of pictorial efforts. A question can then be asked about the merits of theoretical frameworks and pedagogical approaches that sacrifice on the altar of ânatural developmentâ opportunities of young children to grow into individuals who can use with confidence and competence pictorial means for expression and communication throughout their lives. This question is particularly valid in the light of alternative theories that stipulate that development in pictorial representation is in fact a process of learning from cultural models and that these models are absolutely essential in artistic development (Wilson & Wilson, 1977, 1981). Wilson and Wilsonâs research clearly contradicts the idea that âcreative expression comes from within the childâ (Edwards, 1990, p. 66) or that âtalent doesnât need any experience of lifeâ (Klee, quoted by Franciscono, 1998, p. 98). Instead, this work points to the significance of socialization and cultural exposure in the development of pictorial vocabulary that can be used for expressive purposes.
The very concept of artistic development and some longstanding models developed to describe it (e.g., Gaitskell, Hurwitz, & Day, 1982; Lowenfeld & Brittain, 1964; Luquet, 1977) have in fact become a subject of criticism in recent years. Four particular issues have been identified in this collective body of criticism. First, questions have been raised about the appropriateness of unilinear conceptions of development in explaining a wide range of pictorial imagery produced by children, adolescents, and adults. Second, a disparity has been noted between the breadth of the world of art and the narrowness of the focus on visual realism that has served as the endpoint in these developmental models. Third, cultural biases that mark these models have been identified theoretically and empirically. Fourth, insufficient recognition has been paid to the role of other modalities of expression in some forms of pictorial representation (e.g., Duncum, 1986; Golomb, 1994; Kindler & Darras, 1997b, 1998; Korzenik, 1995; Wolf, 1994; Wolf & Perry, 1998). Critics of the traditional stage models of development argued that alternative explanations are necessary to account for the range of pictorial systems that children begin to develop early in life and to engage in research relating to the reasons for selecting specific pictorial solutions. It has been pointed out that even very young children have at their disposal more than one pictorial system and that they choose among them as a function of their representational intentions and purposes (Bremmer & Moore, 1984). The notion of the significance of purposeâor teleologyâof pictorial behavior in the discourse about development of graphic abilities has been at the center of a maplike model of development that has emerged as an alternative to the traditional stage models (Darras & Kindler, 1996; Kindler & Darras, 1997a, 1997b, 1998).
This model regards emergence and development of pictorial imagery as a semiotic process occurring in an interactive social environment that leads to pictorial behaviors that may engage single or multiple modalities of expression. This framework acknowledges psychobiological factors that found development of pictorial activity while emphasizing the role of culture in validating or deselecting specific pictorial systems. It constitutes an attempt to decolonize discourse about artistic development by pointing to the usefulness and value of pictorial repertoires that have remained outside of the acclaim of the Western art world and yet play a significant role in childrenâs lives (Kindler, 1999). In addition to highlighting the dimension of pictorial choices and their relationship to specific purposes of pictorial behavior, the model proposed by Darras and Kindler recognizes frequent interplay of the gestural, vocal-verbal, and graphic actions in the emergence and development of pictorial systems. This is consistent with Parsonsâs (1998) notion about the nature of cognition in the arts. Parsons argues that the prevailing systems approach to cognition identifies the different arts as each being a different symbol system and ârequires thought to stay within the boundaries of a single medium it is dealing with on the assumption that, if it moves from one system to another, it looses its coherenceâ (p. 106). He expresses concern that such understanding of cognition in the arts âtransforms a dimension of difference into a principle of separationâ (p. 106). The repertoire model of development in pictorial imagery moves beyond this principle and accommodates cognitive processes that result in pictorial systems contained within a graphic medium as well as those that cross traditional boundaries of music, drama, and visual arts. This broader conception of development is also more accommodating of cultural factors that define artistic growth than some of the earlier proposed models. The issue of a cultural bias in these earlier theories has vividly surfaced in recent studies that revisited the long-standing notion of the U-shaped curve of artistic development (Davis, 1991, 1997a, 1997b; Gardner, 1980; Gardner & Winner, 1982). The proponents of the Ushaped model argued that while young children exhibit very high levels of creativity in their pictorial work, older children and adolescents suffer from a serious decline in their artistic abilities, which are regained in adulthood only by artistically gifted individuals. The empirical data that confirmed the U-shaped developmental pattern were collected in North America and relied on aesthetic assessments of artistic merit executed by North American and other Western judges.
When this study was recently replicated in settings involving Chinese populations, the U-shaped pattern failed to be confirmed (Kindler, 2001; Pariser & van den Berg, 1997). In none of these recent studies was young childrenâs work considered to be artistically superior to the work of their older peers or nonartistic adults. This suggests that the cultural criteria of artistic merit indeed played a powerful role in defining the very concept of artistic development in research that led to the universality claims of the U-shaped developmental progression. It is important to note that the repertoire model of development does not disprove the U-shape model. In pictorial repertoires that emphasize modernist values, this prediction quite possibly continues to hold true. However, the repertoire model provides also for alternative developmental trajectories in other areas of pictorial pursuits, where technical mastery and preciseness of line, for example, may be of more interest and significance than the imageâs overall expressive quality.
The revised understanding of development in pictorial representation presents an exciting challenge to the field of early childhood art education. It defines a teacherâs role in much more complex terms and places more responsibility on his or her shoulders for ensuring that childrenâs development in this domain does not become unduly constrained by a narrow set of cultural biases and preferences. Valuing and encouraging what we see as childrenâs spontaneous selfdirected explorations need to remain an important consideration in structuring learning environments of young children. However, in order to solve pictorial problems, children also require active assistance in (a) pursuing their own intuitive interests in the visual culture that surrounds them, (b) helping them achieve success in meeting their own pictorial expectations, and (c) exploring their emerging metacognitive abilities in relation to their pictorial production and strategies that can enhance this production. It draws on the use of peeras well as adult-generated examples and readiness to discuss ways in which different systems of pictorial representation can be further mastered and extended to suit childrenâs specific needs and ambitions.
While advocating a much greater sensitivity to and use of cultural models in early childhood education, it is important to clarify that the intention is not to train young children into able performers within a particular system of representation. The answer to early childhood art education will never be presenting children with coloring books or insisting that they copy a particular pictorial schema as the way to learn how to draw. However, a sensitive introduction of activities that help children understand the nature, value, and place of iconic representation and introduce them to a wider range of pictorial systems that function within their culture, supported by conversations and modeling of how different effects can be achieved, will provide children with freedom to choose whatever systems may best serve their purposes and needs in different times in their lives.
It is important to note that especially in the early childhood years childrenâs pictorial intentions may have little to do with creating a picture meeting the modernist standards of âchild art.â Their graphic activity is often just one component of a semiotic process in which sounds, gestures, and words carry a significant portion of meaning. In this context, a teacherâs ability to accept these images as active happenings rather than as objects to be posted on day care or primary classroom walls may help children understand the value of this unique multimodal system of representation and see it as an alternative to other pictorial efforts where, in conformity with the deep-rooted tradition of Western art, images are supposed to stand on their own. Similarly, understanding that telling of a dynamic story may require a child to use different pictorial devices than in the case of a descriptive effort, a greater range of imagery can become socially encouraged and validated (Kindler, 1999). Activities that combine musical, dramatic, and visual art dimensions can further help children grow in pictorial repertoires that rely on multiple modalities of expression and that are often a natural choice of young children who spontaneously combine the gestural, the vocal, and the visual in their everyday play.
The importance of teachersâassistance in a childâs growing understanding of the technical possibilities and limitations of specific artistic media can perhaps be best demonstrated by looking at young childrenâs clay productions. Children as young as 3 years of age who perform in nonintrusive selfexploration environments tend to be much more limited in subject matter, scale, detail, and stylistic diversity in their clay productions than are those who benefit from appropriate instruction (Kindler, 1997). But perhaps most important, sensitive teaching that relies on constructivist approaches and actively guides children toward important discoveries that support their pictorial efforts translates into a sense of accomplishment and pride that sustains childrenâs interest in creative endeavors and encourages them to continue to use and further master this medium of representation. These recommended forms of adult intervention and teaching can be easily integrated into the play format, and skillful and sensitive teachers can make a tremendous difference in childrenâs artistic growth without compromising the child-centered emphasis and the relaxed and playful atmosphere in their early childhood classrooms.
What is needed, however, is a paradigm shift from understanding the concept of developmentally appropriate art education in terms of curricula targeting childrenâs actual levels of attainment to that of creating environments that allow children to perform within their âzones of proximal developmentâ (Vygotsky, 1978).This shift can take place only if we free ourselves from the cultural baggage of romanticized notions of child art and accept that while it certainly can appeal to our aesthetic sensitivities and emotions, there are other territories of pictorial worlds that children should be free to explore, encouraged to learn about, and provided with assistance to master. Our love of childrenâs natural creative efforts should not become detrimental to their artistic growth in the broadest sense of the term.
This revised conception of human development in the area of the pictorial extends the discourse to a new territory inclusive of a wide range of manifestations of visual culture. It still accounts for repertoires that conform to the modernist expectations or give prominence to visual realism, but it also embraces pictorial worlds that rely on different rules, salient attributes, and criteria of excellence. In pluralistic, diversified societies it is especially important to plan early childhood education in ways that do not define artistic growth in hegemonic terms laden by a strong cultural bias. Recent research on artistic development has provided a platform for accommodating this diversity not by proposing a tight universal developmental sequence, but rather by suggesting a range of pictorial repertoires along which childrenâs growth in art can be witnessed and encouraged. The work needs to continue with mapping of the different repertoires and exploration of them in regard to the nature and modes of acquisition of these pictorial systems. Some of them may rely on self-directed discovery; others may require guidance and teaching starting early on in oneâs life. Because the concept of art is constantly broadening and multicultural awareness is leading to a more inclusive dialogue about aesthetic merit and value of different kinds of pictorial imagery, the need to explore diverse avenues of growth and pedagogies that will support it along these multiple dimensions will continue to be an important area of concern and research for the years to come.
Literacy in Early Childhood
From reading readiness to emergent literacy.
There were considerable changes in our understanding of early literacy development in the last decades of the twentieth century. Prior to the late 1970s, a reading readiness paradigm dominated thinking about early literacy development. Central to the concept of reading readiness was the notion that children need to acquire certain prerequisite perceptual and motor skills and to attain a certain mental age before they could begin learning to read (Teale & Sulzby, 1986). Also implicit in reading readiness was the notion that children could not write (i.e., compose) until they had learned to read.
However, in the 1960s researchers began to conduct research on literacy development in children prior to their introduction to formal literacy instruction in school (i.e., Clark, 1976; Durkin, 1966). This groundbreaking body of research revealed that for many children literacy development begins long before their formal schooling and that, indeed, some children entered school as competent and independent readers. This early work precipitated a flurry of research in a number of aspects of literacy. Clay (1979) and Holdaway (1979) showed that young childrenâs reading behaviors developed along a continuum from initial, rough approximations of reenacting texts to accurate decoding of print. Research on emergent literacy included work on childrenâs writing as well their reading. For example, Bissex (1980), Chomsky (1977), and Read (1975) investigated young childrenâs writing development and were able to show that childrenâs early attempts at spelling were not random but were quite systematic and demonstrated a sophisticated and developmental understanding of phoneme-grapheme relationships. Working from within a Piagetian tradition, Ferreiro and Teberosky (1982) demonstrated that young children formed hypotheses about representing speech through writing that they modified as they refined their knowledge. From this collective body of work with young children sprung the theory of emergent literacy (Clay, 1966). Sulzby and Teale (1991) defined emergent literacy thus:
[A]n emergent literacy perspective ascribes to the child the role of constructor of his or her own literacy. Unlike previous work, the central issues now being addressed are the nature of the childâs contributions (i.e., individual construction), the role of the social environment in the process (i.e., social construction), and the interface between the two. (p. 729)
The research in emergent literacy had a profound impact on our understanding of both how literacy is learned and how it could be taught. For example, many of the tenets of whole language, a movement that flourished in the 1980s and 1990s, could be traced to this work.As this body of research on emergent literacy grew, so did the number of questions in four specific areas: the generalizability of the research, the presumed similarity between oral language learning and the development of literacy, the impact of culture on literacy development, and the importance of storybook reading.
Questions about the generalizability were focused on both the methodologies that were used as well as on the target populations under study.AsAdams (1990) pointed out, much of the research in emergent literacy has been limited to âcase studies, chronologies and descriptionsâ (p. 336). Heath and Thomas (1984) pointed to the fact that in many of these studies the children on whom the research was conducted were the children of âparent-academicsâ (Heath & Thomas, 1984, p. 51) with their own children.
Questions were also raised about the presumed similarity between oral language development and literacy learning. For many researchers (i.e., Holdaway, 1979), literacy development was portrayed as a natural process that paralleled the development of language acquisition. A number of critiques attempted to refute this notion by pointing out the ways in which literacy acquisition was fundamentally different from first-language acquisition (i.e., Luke, Baty, & Stehbens, 1989); in particular, other research showed that in contrast to natural language development, some children did benefit from direct instruction in literacy (e.g., Adams, 1990).
Increasingly, researchers have begun to address questions of social, cultural, and linguistic diversity. When many of the original studies on emergent literacy were replicated with diverse populations, the initial positive findings on the effects of whole languageâemergent literacy approaches were not replicated. For example, Anderson and Matthews (1999) and Elster (1994) found that children from working-class homes did not always follow the developmental trajectory of storybook reading that Sulzby (1985) found with the children from middle-class homes in her study. Other researchers have found that parents from diverse cultural groups support their young childrenâs literacy development differently (i.e., Anderson, 1995; Reyes, 1992). For example, many of the Chinese-Canadian parents in Andersonâs study expressed very strong opposition to many of the literacy practices considered sacrosanct within an emergent literacy paradigm. These practices included accepting the use of invented spelling and childrenâs pretend reading behaviors. The initial position paper by the National Association for the Education of Young Children on developmentally appropriate practices in literacy reflected a more or less homogenous prescription for a universal application of the principles of emergent literacy; it was later revised in recognition of cultural diversity in literacy learning (Bredekamp, 1997).
Related to questions of culture, researchers and practitioners have also raised questions about the relative importance of storybook reading in the early years. Because some of the earlier research with precocious readers (i.e., Clark, 1976; Durkin, 1966; Wells, 1985) found that storybook reading was often a part of their daily literacy routine, the general assumption was drawn that this activity was a necessary prerequisite that applied to all children (Pellegrini, 1991). This sweeping assumption tended to prevail even though in some cultures where storybook reading to children is practically nonexistent, most children in that culture still can achieve literate competence (Mason, 1992). Further, a major meta-analysis has suggested that storybook reading accounts for only about 8% of the variance in reading (Scarborough & Dobrich, 1994).
Despite these issues and concerns, the concept of emergent literacy has fundamentally altered the way that literacy learning in early childhood is conceived (Adams, 1990). Consistent with the basic premises of emergent literacy, for example, it is now widely acknowledged that literacy learning should be meaningful, functional, and contextualized and that a great deal of literacy learning occurs before children enter school. Research on literacy continues to focus both on some traditional areas of debate (e.g., the relative importance of phonics instruction) as well as on some emerging areas of interest (e.g., family literacy). It is to these two perspectives on literacy in early childhood education that we now turn.
The Role of Phonological Awareness
Perhaps no other single issue in early literacy has received as much attention over the decades than has phonological awareness or the ability to segment the speech stream into its constituent parts (e.g., phonemes). For example, many literacy researchers and educators see phonemic awareness as a prerequisite to learning to read and write (Bradley & Bryant, 1983; Stanovich, 1986). In contrast, other researchers (Malicky & Norman, 1999) acknowledge that while a certain level of phonemic awareness is needed to learn to decode print, children can become more phonemically aware through the process of learning to read and write. Mustafa (1997) and others argue that phonemic awareness is but one component of a more general competency in language and that it relates, for example, to vocabulary size. The debate continues whether phonological development is best achieved through direct instruction or through more informal, meaningful, and contextualized activities such as word play, rhymes, games, and songs.
Perhaps the most contentious issue in early literacy over the past half century has been the role of systematic instruction in the alphabetic code, commonly known as phonics (Chall, 1967). Although the debate still rages, especially in the United States, a more moderate position or middle ground is evolving. Recent reports have called for direct instruction in the phonics code but in contextualized ways with ample opportunity for children to apply the skill being learned in meaningful situations (Pressley et al., 1998; Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998).
The relationship between phonemic awareness in writing as well as in reading has been the focus of much research in recent decades. For example, whereas proponents of whole language once argued that children learned to spell through writing and âinventedâ spelling, researchers have shown that structured word study could also help young children become more proficient spellers (Templeton & Morris, 1999). These researchers have also shown that although it was once thought that learning to spell essentially consisted of memorizing lists of isolated words, efficient spellers use layers of information to help them learn the orthography. During the heyday of the process writing movement (e.g., Graves, 1983), personal writing such as journals, diaries, and so forth was heavily promoted. An underlying assumption of the writing process movement was that writing skills are generic and transferable from one context to another. However, researchers are now beginning to find that such is not the case and that each particular genre requires specialized knowledge and forms. Chapmanâs (1995) research has shown that children who are exposed to a variety of genres in their daily experiences will also incorporate the specific features of different genres into their writing at a young age.
The debate on the relative importance of phonemic awareness in children and of phonics instruction with young children manifests itself in a variety of curriculum debates as well. There has been a general trend away from the use of programs based on behavioristic psychology (e.g., DISTAR) and toward approaches that are based more on principles of cognitive psychology. One widely known and widely applied curriculum program, Reading Recovery, was developed by Marie Clay in New Zealand. In Reading Recovery the children who have the most difficulty in reading and writing receive one half hour of individualized tutoring per day with teachers specially trained in Reading Recovery techniques. Despite the ubiquity of this program and even though generally favorable research reports on its effectiveness, it has received criticism on a number of points, including the following: The program is overly structured and inflexible (Dudley-Marling & Murphy, 1997); it does include a specific phonological awareness component (Chapman, Tumner, & Prochnow, 1998); it does not remediate the needs of those children most in need of support (Santa & Hoien, 1999); its initial gains wash out after children have attained expected grade-level proficiency in reading and, in the terminology of the program, are discontinued (Chapman et al., 1998); it does not lead to the staff development that its originator and proponents argue would happen (Center, Whedall, Freeman, Outhred, & McNaught, 1995); and it expends scarce resources without being any more effective than alternate, small-group tutoring (Santa & Hoien, 1999). Whereas the concept of multiple literacies (the notion that meaning is encoded in myriad ways other than print) has been around for some time, Luke and Luke (2001) argued that mainstream thinking in the early literacy field still privileges print literacy. That is, despite the advances in technology that allow for new ways of constructing and communicating meaning, the focus in early literacy still seems to be on decoding print. This fact is evidenced by recent trends in some parts of the United States to prescribe decodable texts for early reading instruction and to emphasize basic reading and writing skills. Decodable texts are texts written specifically for beginning reading instruction. They have a very high level of grapheme-phoneme agreement, which proponents argue helps children learn to crack the code. Critics argue that such texts use contrived, unnatural language that will not engender a desire to read in children. Thus, at this point in time it appears unlikely that curricula and instruction in early literacy will begin to reflect in any significant way a multiple-literacies perspective in the near future.
Family Literacy
Another trend that will deserve increasing attention in the coming years is the proliferation of family literacy programs. Taylor (1983) initially coined the term family literacy to describe the myriad ways in which the middle-class families with whom she worked embedded literacy in their daily lives. Taylorâs portrayal of the family as a site where considerable literacy learning takes place generated substantial interest in this phenomenon. Educators have subsequently been involved in developing family literacy programs that are intended to help parents support their young childrenâs literacy development. Critics of family literacy programs contend that underlying many such programs are some very troubling deficit assumptions about families that are poor where relatively little literacy is observed. There appears to be some kind of causal or predictive relationship between a familyâs poverty and their lower levels of literacy practices in the home. Further, the argument has been that the literacy that does occur in these homes is not the ârightâ kind of literacy that will adequately prepare the children for school. The criticism has also been raised that many of these programs are gender-biased in that they are targeted specifically at childrenâs mothers. Mace (1998) has argued that
[t]he evidence of a literacy âproblemâ in industrialized countries with mass schooling systems has revealed that schools alone cannot meet this need. Families must be recruited to do their bit too. This is where the spotlight falls on the mother. She it is who must ensure that the young child arrives at school ready for school literacy and preferably already literate. (p. 5)
Whereas play has traditionally been seen within the early childhood community as the medium through which children learn (see also the section on the role of play in early childhood education), it is only recently that researchers have begun to investigate the role of play in young childrenâs literacy learning. For example, using an ethnographic research approach, Anderson (1995) documented the many ways in which one child incorporated various forms of workplace literacy into her play. In their work in day care settings, Neuman and Roskos (1993) found that childrenâs interest in literacy and their interaction with literacy materials were enhanced when appropriate literacy materials were placed in different play centers.They also suggested that âmore challenging and complex language use may be produced in play environments that are literacy basedâ (p. 221). These authors cautioned that the literacy knowledge that children develop through play may not be sufficient for literacy acquisition. However, they argue that childrenâs âcomprehension of the act of literacyâ is developed through play (p. 221). Purcell-Gates (1996) proposed that this comprehension is crucial to childrenâs literacy development. She commented, âGrasping the signifying nature of print and the many ways it can function in peoples lives has been called the big picture . . . and is basic to any further knowledge about the forms and conventions of written languageâ (p. 422). However, the importance of play in literacy development is not universally embraced, as the pendulum appears to be swinging backâat least in some quarters in the United Statesâtoward formal instruction in phonics. Thus, the zeitgeist of literacy theory continues to include simultaneously both advocates of child-centered, emergent literacy approaches and those who focus on the importance of phonemic segmentation and code breaking that can only be acquired through direct formal instruction.
Concluding Thoughts on Literacy in Early Childhood
Increasing trends of globalization are beginning to raise important policy issues in the area of early literacy. For example, in Vancouver, Canada, over 50% of the children entering kindergarten speak English as a second language, and there are over 50 different foreign languages that these children collectively bring to school. In a landmark report, Snow et al. (1998) proposed that initial literacy instruction should be in the childâs first language. Although this might be possible where there are concentrations of families and children with a common language, it would not be possible for the 50% of children entering Vancouver kindergartens every year who speak English as a second language as well as for countless others such as those in the schools in Canada alluded to earlier. The issue of literacy in oneâs own first language in addition to literacy in English will continue to grow in complexity as society becomes increasingly diverse.
Music in Early Childhood
All children are born with some degree of music ability. A major challenge that confronts ECE music educators and researchers alike is to determine the relative importance of (a) a childâs innate music aptitude, (b) the music environments that contextualize the childâs life, and (c) the roles of indirect and direct instruction in music in the early years. This discussion briefly addresses each of these issues from theoretical, empirical, and applied perspectives.
It is widely understood that a childâs music aptitude has an ideal chance for optimal development if parents, caregivers, and teachers provide a varied and rich music environment for a child early in life (Brand, 1982, 1985, 1986; Gordon, 1990; Pond, 1981; Simons, 1986). In fact, some researchers have hypothesized that children can suffer severe music developmental delays when music is not a frequent part of their home and school environments. Beyond enhancing the development of a childâs music abilities, Doxey and Wright (1990) also reported significant positive relationships between music and mathematics abilities in their study of music cognition and general intelligence. Subsequent research conducted over the past decade has also reported findings of enhanced cognitive development and spatial-temporal reasoning as the result of rich, stimulating preschool music activities (Rauscher et al., 1997; Shaw, 1999). The research evidence strongly suggests that an enriched music environmentâa combination of quality, quantity, and a variety of music experiencesâin the first several years of a childâs life can impact strongly on a childâs success in and enjoyment of music, as well as in other areas of cognitive development.
Innate Audiation and Learned Music Skill Development
Although environmental factors can contribute to a childâs success in music, the most important factor for potential growth in music is the childâs innate music aptitude, an ability that can now be evaluated by a reliable, standardized test (Gordon, 1978, 1979, 1986a, 1986b, 1989). In contemporary terms, standardized music aptitude tests measure a childâs ability to audiate music. Audiation is the innate human ability to create and recreate music, to conceptualize and comprehend and to compare music that has been heard in the past with music being heard in the present, and to music that may be heard in the future (Gouzouasis, 1992). Humans sing, chant, move, play instruments, and respond to music that they audiate. Humans audiate when they listen to music, recall music, read music, improvise music, and compose music (Gordon, 1986b).
In all of these ways and more, children, adolescents, and adults play music and play with music. Music play may occur consciously or unconsciously, spontaneously or planned, in unstructured or structured environments, and in informal or formal contexts, and may either be child initiated or adult initiated. Fundamental to all of these forms of music comprehension, production, and enjoyment is the childâs ability to audiate. A childâs ability to audiate may greatly influence the way in which he or she organizes the physical aspect of music (i.e., sound) in terms of melody, harmony, form, texture, and timbre and may greatly influence the way a child produces music through singing, chanting, moving, and performing on instruments. Of the many different elements of music, the tonal and rhythmic aspects can be most reliably measured in young children (Gordon, 1979).
Research has shown that childrenâs audiation abilities fluctuate during the early childhood years (Flohr, 1981; Gouzouasis, 1987, 1991; Jessup, 1984; Levinowitz, 1985; Zimmerman, 1986). It is widely accepted that children by the age of 9 years can learn to play an instrument, to dance, to sing, to read, to improvise, and to compose music. Although music achievement can certainly continue to develop in and beyond middle childhood, after age 9 the degree of music achievement a child may attain is seriously attenuated by his or her stabilized level of audiation ability. Indeed, there is ample evidence to support the notion that even young adults can be taught music listening strategies and instrumental performance that enable them to maximize their basic audiation ability in a variety of music contexts (Gouzouasis, 2000). Ultimately, however, their success in those music activities will be restricted by their ability to make connections between a broad range of acquired music skills and their innate audiation ability.
Central to developing a childâs music ability is the importance of objectively assessing each childâs music aptitude level. This allows the childâs teacher to teach to the childâs music strengths, thereby facilitating the childâs level of music achievement. In the early 1980s a number of researchers began to scrutinize traditional music-teaching practices in an attempt to observe how very young children acquire music in natural and noninstructional settings. This research represented a sharp departure from earlier top-down curriculum approaches that had been generated primarily by Western European composers and practitioners (Andress, 1986, 1989; Boswell, 1986; Goetze, Cooper, & Brown, 1990). This was in stark contrast to traditional, historical recapitulation models of music teaching and learning. Within this body of work four specific areas of focus (discussed later) are (a) the development of a childâs singing voice, (b) the relationship between music and movement, (c) the implications of âhotâ and âcoolâ music media on children, and (d) childrenâs singing songs with and without words.
The research on singing voice has revealed that two- and three-pitch diatonic stepwise patterns are easier to sing than are arpeggiated patterns (Gordon, 1990; Gouzouasis, 1991; Guilmartin & Levinowitz, 1989â2000; Jersild & Bienstock, 1931,1934).Becausepitchedvocalglidesareeasilyproduced by children as young as 1 month of age, stepwise patterns are a natural feature in the developmental sequence of pitched vocalization. In contrast, arpeggiated patterns require more vocal precision. The research demonstrated that when young children move their bodies to music, it is important that they experience the free exploration of weight, flow, space, and time with the whole body and various body parts without the expectation that they attempt to coordinate their movements with an externally imposed steady beat (Gouzoausis, 1991). Because singing is in many ways a movement activity, the acquisition of a sense of steady beat is fundamental to all aspects of music learning, and it needs to be nurtured in a variety of play activities, with and without music accompaniment.
Gouzouasis (1987) drew metaphoric allusions between McLuhanâs idea of âcoolâ and âhotâ media and the effects of two types of music accompaniment and nonaccompaniment on the singing achievement of 5-year-old children. There is evidence that nonaccompaniment facilitates the learning of songs, especially for children who possess low tonal audiation ability. Ironically, because of the prevalence of heavily textured, hot music learning contexts in contemporary music, twenty-first-century culture may be depriving young children of informal exposure to music experiences presented in developmentally appropriate learning contexts, or cool music media. Further, children who possess high tonal audiation ability can be expected to acquire a singing voice and sing songs consistently better than children who possess low tonal audiation ability, regardless of style and accompaniment texture. Thus, children who have inherently lower levels of audiation may have their music potential additionally compromised by the intensity and texture of much of the hot music media to which they are exposed.
A related application of that line of research led to the practice of singing songs without words with very young children (Gordon, 1990; Gouzouasis, 1987; Levinowitz, 1987). Very young children who possess few linguistic skills tend to respond very positively to singing activities with a neutral syllable such as âbahâ or âtooâ than to songs with many words, sentences, and lyrics. Many young children sing the songs with which they are most familiar in tune, and they sing less familiar songs out of tune. That may be because they are better able to concentrate their listening, audiation, and vocal-production abilities on the music aspects of the song rather than the text, a characteristic that seems to be true of children who possess low to average music aptitude (Gouzouasis, 1987). Children seem to benefit most from singing songs without words because an emphasis on learning the words of the song can distract and detract from their attention to the music content. As a childâs language skills develop, song texts (i.e., lyrics) seem to interfere less with vocal development. A mixture of songs with and without words should be of benefit to older children (Levinowitz, 1987). Parents, caregivers, and teachers may still teach their favorite songs, but they can teach them using a neutral syllable instead of text. The ability of a child to sing one or more short phrases of a song, with or without text and in tune, is evidence that he or she has emerged from the tonal babble stage.
Music Acquisition and Language Acquisition
Although music is not a language, it is acquired in a manner and context similar to those in which language is learned (Gouzouasis & Taggart, 1995). The mechanisms that a young child uses to produce sounds are the same for both music and language. The throat, mouth, nose, lungs, and diaphragm are used in a variety of ways to produce meaningful music and linguistic sounds. For both music and language, sounds may vary in loudness, pitch, duration, and stress. When sound (or, technically, phones ) in the form of vowels and consonants ( segmentals ), is organized into morphemesâthe smallest meaning-based unit in languageâthe sound is expressed as a linguisticmedium(Jakobson,1968).Similarly,whensoundin the form of pitches and durations is organized into tonal and rhythm patterns, it may be considered a music medium (Gouzouasis, 1987). Young children are encouraged to produce linguistic sounds from the time they enter our world through what psycholinguists have described as motherese, a reciprocal and contingent interaction between parent and infant (Broen, 1972; Cross, 1977; Newport, Gleitman, & Glietman, 1977; Phillips, 1970; Snow, 1977). Motherese is characterized by language that is simplified and limited in vocabulary, with words pronounced slowly with careful and exaggerated enunciation and in a repetitive manner. Motherese makes use of three different components of language: semantics (vocabulary), syntax (the predictability of sentence patterns), and pragmatics (the social and instrumental uses of language). Although there appears to be a similar babbling and mimicking stage in music, most young children are not exposed to a corresponding music variant of motherese (Holohan, 1984), perhaps because a shared system of semantics, syntax, and pragmatics has not yet been worked out between parent and infant.
Research has shown, however, that the music babble stage has at least two parts, rhythm and tonality, which seem to operate independently of each other (Gordon, 1990). Although the music babble stage can last essentially from birth until the child is 6 or 7 years old, some children have been observed to leave the babble stage completely as early as 24 months, depending on their music aptitude and environmental influences. A child can still be engaged in tonal babble after he or she is out of rhythm babble, and vice versa. On their own, children may babble by singing short patterns and experimenting (i.e., playing) with their voices. Children who babble on a tonal level seem to be singing in a monotone; that is, it centers in a narrow range around one pitch. The songs that such children sing are often unrecognizable to an adult, and the children may be unaware that their singing is different from that produced by adults. Although tonal babble may sound amusical to an adult, it should be encouraged through parental play and imitation that can begin as soon as an infant begins to produce pitched sounds (Stark, 1977; Stark, Rose, & McClagen, 1975). Many children who babble early and often tend be identified as those who learn to sing with the music syntax of an adult earlier than children who babble considerably less.
When a child is at the stage of using tonal babble, rhythm babble, or both, all music instruction should be informal (Gordon, 1990). Children who emerge from the tonal babble stage are able to audiate music with tonal syntax and are able to audiate and sing music with a sense of tonalityâwith a sense of how patterns are organized. They learn to sing what they audiate and to audiate what they sing (Gordon, 1990). In essence, they learn to coordinate their listening (perception) of music, their audiation (conception), and their vocal production of music (through their breathing, diaphragm, and vocal chords) in order to sing with a sense of tonality.
Play and Music Play
In many ways babble is an elemental form of play in music. Play, imagination, and creativity are naturally rooted in music activities in early childhood. In fact, one may consider music itself as a form of play. Humans play music. Young children play in a variety of activities while listening to music, and they play musically in a variety of contextsâwith their voices, with their bodies, with props, and with music instruments. Observational research reveals that children spontaneously accompany their play with music. It is a simple form of multitasking, in that young children possess the abilities to attend to more than one activity while engaged in conscious and unconscious music making. Music play may be child initiated or caregiver initiated. Both forms are equally as diverse, rich, and valuable in learning. Moreover, music play may occur in both structured and unstructured and formal and informal settings (Gouzouasis, 1991, 1994). An understanding of the multifaceted relationships between play and music is central to both theories of music acquisition and learning. Moorehead and Pond (1981), Pond (1978), and Littleton (1999) provided brilliant insight to this topic, which sadly has been neglected by education researchers and psychologists.
Concluding Thoughts on Music in Early Childhood Education
Achild who is developmentally delayed in music learning deserves the same type of specialized instruction that a child receives who is developmentally delayed in some other aspect of learning. Although it seems that various forms of media made positive contributions to the proliferation of music in the twentieth century, the developmentally detrimental aspects and negative influences of electronic and digital media are too numerous to mention in this research paper (Gouzouasis, 2000). One may begin by scrutinizing the use of music in childrenâs television programming and the lack of quality music for young children on radio, recordings, and new forms of digital media (e.g., the Internet). Much of what is marketed as music for young children is actually developmentally appropriate for older children and is composed to appeal to parents and caregivers. Those factors are compounded by the gross commercialization of early childhood music instruction by corporate early childhood music trainers. From a perspective informed by both research and praxis, it is arguable that any music instruction is better than none at all.
Play in Early Childhood
âThe play of children may strike us at times as fragile and charming, rowdy and boisterous, ingenuous, just plain silly, or disturbingly perceptive in its portrayals of adult actions and attitudesâ (Garvey, 1977, p. 1). A plethora of scholars have focused on these and other aspects of playâs characteristic forms, which has led to a proliferation of theories to account for the origins, properties, and functions of these forms. Thus, to begin to understand play, in all of its ambiguities, requires multiple perspectives. This section provides a brief review of selected theories and research on childrenâs play, highlighting the paradoxical nature of the phenomenon.
Defining Play
Given the protean nature of play, defining it has proven problematic in the literature. In Western cultures, our understanding of play has been influenced most significantly by shared attitudes about what play is not; for example, âplay is not work, play is not serious, play is not productiveâtherefore, play is not importantâ (Schwartzman, 1991, p. 214). Caillois (1961) argued that âin effect, play is essentially a separate occupation, carefully isolated from the rest of life and generally, is engaged in within precise limits of time and placeâ (p. 6). Denzin (1980a), on the other hand, proposed that the world of play is not distinct from everyday taken-for-granted reality, but that it occurs in the immediately experienced here and now. He also stressed that on an a priori basis play cannot be distinguished from other everyday interactions, including conversation and other activities of habit. Other definitions of play include that play is pleasurable and enjoyable, has no extrinsic goals, and is spontaneous and voluntary (Garvey, 1977); that play is free, separate, uncertain, unproductive, governed by rules, and full of make-believe (Caillois, 1961); and that play is a story that children tell themselves about themselves (Geertz, 1973). The ambiguity of these definitions reflects Western societyâs struggle over how to conceptualize and value play.
Perspectives on Play
The diversity and ambiguity inherent in definitions of play have resulted in broad conceptualizations of the forms and functions of the phenomenon. These conceptualizations include play and cognition, psychoanalytic theory, educational perspectives, play and literacy, and play as communication, among others.
Traditional Theories of Play and Cognition. Child development theorists and researchers have attempted to explain the relationship between play and childrenâs cognitive development. Two major theorists, Jean Piaget and Leo Vygotsky, are perhaps the most noted for furthering our understanding of this relationship. Piaget (1962) believed that children gain knowledge through the dual processes of assimilation and accommodation. In assimilation, children take in information from their experiences in the external world, which is then integratedâ assimilatedâ into existing mental structures. Because childrenâs cognitive structures are often inadequate to incorporate new information, they must learn to change or accommodate their mental structures to better accept information that is inconsistent with what they already know. Typically, the opposing forces of assimilation and accommodation must work in tandem to reach a state of equilibrium. The activity of play, however, is unique because children are able to suspend reality and make the world adapt to them; thus, assimilation assumes primacy over accommodation (Saracho & Spodek, 1995). Piaget (1962) offered an account of childrenâs unfolding cognitive process by identifying three stages of play: sensorimotor, symbolic, and games with rules. According to Piaget, each stage is part of a sequential order in a childâs development. The first stage involves repetitive actions that focus on physical activity. In the second stage children use their symbolic abilities to create and act out stories. The final stage involves the social conventions of rules in games. The cross-cultural applicability of Piagetâs theory, however, has been widely questioned because his data are based primarily on observations of White, middle-class children in Western society (see, e.g., Denzin, 1980b).
Vygotskyâs view of the relationship between play and development differs significantly from Piagetâs. Vygotsky (1976) argued that childrenâs play extends their cognitive development. Specifically, children have a âzone of proximal development,â a range of tasks between those that can be completed independently and those that can be mastered only through the mediation of adults or more competent peers. Vygotsky believed that social interaction with more competent others is critical to a childâs cognitive development because it is the social context of socialization experiences that shapes the thought processes of the young child. The emphasis on social interaction renders Vygotskyâs theory of play and cognition applicable across social and cultural contexts.
Psychoanalytic Theories of Play. Freud (1909) used the pretend play of children as the medium for understanding their conscious and unconscious wishes and fears. His theory of play was based on the idea of internal conflict, and he postulated that play is cathartic for children because it allows them to resolve negative feelings that may result from traumatic experiences. For instance, a child who has experienced the trauma of an accident and must be taken to the hospital, away from the safety and familiarity of home, may later play and replay various hospital scenes in order to cope with feelings of fear and pain. Other theorists have modified Freudâs psychoanalytic theory and have related play to wish fulfillment, anxiety, and ego processes (see Takhvar, 1988, for a review). Erikson, in contrast, believed that children use play to dramatize the past, present, and future, and to resolve conflicts that they experience in each stage of their development. Peller (1952) thought that childrenâs imitations of life in their play were caused by feelings of love, admiration, fear, and aggression.
Educational Perspectives on Play. Since the early nineteetn century, educators have observed the transition from play to learning as children struggle to leave the world of play at home to enter the world of learning at school. The idea that play could be used as an articulation of teaching practices and curricula was first put into practice by Pestalozzi (1827), who believed that young children could be educated to develop an inquiring approach to things and words. He developed a pedagogy that encouraged the development of childrenâs activity that was built on their potential for moral and aesthetic discernment through reflection.
It was Froebelâs modification of Pestalozziâs theory, however, that became a medium for learning within the context of schooling. Froebel proposed that âplay is the purest, most spiritual activity, and at the same time, typical of human life as a wholeâof the inner hidden natural life of [human beings]â (Froebel, 1885, pp. 86â87). Froebel also suggested that children attempt to maintain continuity in their lives by bringing playful activity to their formalized learning experiences. For example, he argued that play allows children to achieve mastery over many aspects of themselves and their environment through symbolic enactment of roles, exploration of feelings, and interaction with others (Garvey, 1977). Such themes are typically repeated during several play episodes, which suggests that play is also cathartic for children because it allows them to reexperience and thereby resolve or master a difficult situation.
Froebel observed that the way in which children play often reveals their inner struggles (Adelman, 1990) and that play is often the primary means for children to learn social expectations, attempt to understand culturally appropriate behaviors, struggle to learn to manage emotions, and gain access to the techniques and skills of the world in which they live (Michelet, 1986). According to Froebel, it is essentially the childâs whole personality that can be seen to be involved in play. He in fact contended that to understand the whole child, it is crucial to understand that the inextricable link between the inner and the outer parts of childrenâs play have a visible and metaphysical aspect (Adelman, 1990). All of Froebelâs activities and materials symbolized spiritual meanings that he wanted children to gain (Saracho & Spodek, 1995), and the activities he developed were based on observations he made of German peasant children.
Maria Montessori (1965) also conceptualized her teaching methods from the natural play activities of children. She developed her methods by bringing into the classroom materials she was designing. She watched children play freely with them and then abstracted what she considered the essential elements of the play. Free play, however, was discouraged after she decided how the materials could be used best (Montessori, 1965). Montessori contended that by using her materials, âchildren could sharpen their abilities to gather and organize their sensory impressions in order to better absorb knowledgeâ (Saracho & Spodek, 1995, p. 130).
It was the advent of the progressive kindergarten movement, however, that provided the basis for contemporary educational uses of play (Saracho & Spodek, 1995). The movement was spearheaded by Dewey, who broke from the colonial view that children should avoid play to become more work oriented as they matured (Hartley & Goldenson, 1963). Dewey argued that play could be used to help children construct their understanding of the world, and that through play, children would learn to function at higher levels of consciousness and action (Saracho & Spodek, 1995). Play in Deweyâs terms, however, was still not a free activity. Instead, teachers were to use play to create an environment to nurture and enhance childrenâs mental and moral growth (Dewey, 1916).
Play and Literacy
Understanding the connection between childrenâs knowledge and use of literacy and their play behavior is a focus of current research. Research in the area of play and literacy is grounded in the theoretical work of Piaget (1962) and Vygotsky (1976). Both theorists compare the use of symbols in symbolic play and literacy. Piaget described play as being largely assimilative and viewed it as a reflection of the childâs cognitive development. He maintained that during play children demonstrate a mental distancing from what is real in the here and now through their symbolic representations of people, places, things, and actions. Moreover, he suggested that play may serve as a catalyst for the childâs emerging literacy skills. Vygotsky (1976) described how children represent literacyâ from the gesture to the written wordâas a unified process. He also believed that the social meaning of marks on a piece of paper is rooted in how a childâs indicatory gestures are responded to during play. Unlike Piaget, however, Vygotsky proposed that play is the primary factor in fostering childrenâs development, liberating their thoughts from specific contexts and from the literal meanings of concrete actions and uses of objects. He also theorized that the ability to engage in symbolic play enables children to develop a variety of represented (symbolic) meanings that serve as the basis for later success in literacy.
Other theorists have also hypothesized about the relationship between play and literacy. Bruner (1983), for example, advised that childrenâs early literacy development should be an integral aspect of play-based experiences that support childrenâs ideas, purposes, and social interpretations. He cautioned, however, that structuring and organizing play for educational purposes often results in âtaking the action away from the childâ (p. 62). Others, such as Donaldson (1978) and Heath (1983), pointed out that engaging in play and learning to read and write demand similar cognitive abilities; that is, through interacting with print, the learner moves from episodes of the here and now to settings that are decontexualized within text.
Theoretical studies on the play-literacy connection have provided the impetus for a number of empirical studies. There are two main strands of research in this area, both of which focus on the parallel representational processes involved in literacy and play. One strand of research examines how to enhance literacy use and knowledge through symbolic play (see, e.g., Jacob, 1984; Miller, Fernie, & Kantor, 1992; Neuman & Roskos, 1992, 1993; Roskos, 1988). Schrader (1991) and others (see, e.g., Roskos & Neuman, 1993) have been interested in the ways that children pretend to write as part of their dramatic play. Michaels (1981) examined childrenâs sharing-time narrative styles and differential access to literacy. Dyson (1997) studied the reciprocal relationship between childrenâs writing and the superhero dramas and discussions that followed in the classroom and on the playground.
Asecond strand of research has emphasized childrenâs use of language in symbolic play. Much of the research in this area has demonstrated that when teachers and parents become involved with childrenâs pretend play, there are positive increases in childrenâs literacy, language, reading, and writing (Bloch & Pellegrini, 1989; Christie, 1991; Galda & Pellegrini, 1985; Goelman & Jacobs, 1994; Pellegrini, 1984; Pellegrini & Galda, 1998).
Play as Communication
It was Bateson (1955, 1972) who first suggested that play was a paradoxical form of communication. He argued that play is socially situated and characterized by the production and exchange of paradoxical statements about people, objects, activities, situations, and the relationships among these. Bateson (1972) saw play as an ancient form of communication, which was based on his notion about animalsâplay fighting. He argued that because animals have no negatives (i.e., they cannot say ânoâ), negative behaviors such as âbiting must be illustrated positively by not really bitingâ (KellyByrne, 1989, p. 246). Playful nipping must be communicated as not really biting, even though it stands for biting. Thus, play is not merely play, but is also a message about itself. In Batesonâs words, âthese actions in which we now engage do not denote what these actions for which they stand denote. . . . The playful nip denotes the bite but it does not denote what would be denoted by the biteâ (1955, p. 41). Play is therefore paradoxical because the behavior engaged in by the players is at once real and not real. Thus, the child playing mother is both a mother and yet not a mother. Other playersâ awareness of this paradox is, according to Bateson (1955), a form of metacommunication.
Both Garvey (1977) and Schwartzman (1978) have expanded Batesonâs perceptions of play. Their body of work has concentrated on how children organize and communicate about make-believe, how the message âthis is playâ is signaled, and how play is initiated, sustained, and concluded. Schwartzman (1978) extended Garveyâs work by focusing on the meanings children attached to the texts they generated in play. Following the work of Geertz (1972) and Ehrmann (1968), Schwartzman (1978) argued that childrenâs pretend play could be analyzed âas a text in which players act as both subjects and the objects of their created play eventâ (p. 232). In her interpretations of play texts, she emphasized the importance of social and cultural contexts and suggested that play was very much about dominance and manipulation. Schwartzmanâs understanding of hierarchical relationships in play âillustrates the weaving of the childrenâs social histories with the texts of their play and the relationship of both elements to the wider sociocultural context of the place of children and their hierarchical ranking in a variety of institutions such as the family and schoolâ (Kelly-Byrne, 1989, p. 12).As children weave their social histories into their play texts, they fashion their relationships with others and express in subtleâ and sometimes not so subtleâways their individual perceptions of the world around them (Bakhtin, 1981). In other words, play is a medium through which children communicate and make sense of who they are in relation to others.
Closing Thoughts on Play in Early Childhood Education
Play has been defined and examined across a number of disciplines including biology, psychology, linguistics, sociology, anthropology, art, literature, and leisure studies. It has provide the means to delve into childrenâs inner thoughts and feelings, extend their cognitive growth and development, enhance their language and literacy development, and understand how they communicate their perceptions of the world. Such diversity in the interpretation of play both augments and constrains our understanding of the phenomenon, making reconciliation among play scholars difficult. Perhaps the best point of convergence is for both researchers and teachers to adopt an interdisciplinary approach focusing on the meanings that children themselves attach to play. Children are, after all, the ultimate authorities on the subject.
Diversity in Early Childhood: Individual Exceptionality and Cultural Pluralism
Giftedness and early childhood education.
Gifted children are found in every community. These very able youngsters reason, create, speak, write, read, interact, feel, make music, or move in ways that distinguish them from their age peers. They are developmentally advanced in one or more areas of human accomplishment and often demonstrate early indicators of their gifts as infants. As they enter ECE settings, they may encounter mixed reactions to their abilities that have consequences for the educational programs they are offered. Appropriate educational experiences for young gifted children are impacted by different macrosystem factors: the ideologies of parents, teachers, and society as a whole; the education system; and psychology, the discipline that provides a knowledge base for understanding and educating young gifted children and important directions for research. This discussion on giftedness presents some of the issues relevant to ensuring appropriate early childhood education for gifted children within the context of contemporary psychological knowledge. It also summarizes directions for research that will have implications for gifted children in ECE classrooms, for although the early years are acknowledged as critically formative, a significant body of research focused on gifted young children has yet to accrue (Robinson, 1993, 2000).

Prevalent Ideologies and the Education System
A number of powerful ideological factors may influence how young gifted children are educated. In some cases, ideology is combined with systemic factors, resulting in school administrative structures that are not responsive to individual childrenâs needs. First, an age-grade mindset may prevail, resulting in the belief that, for example, all 6-year-olds belong in first grade for both academic and social reasons (Robinson & Robinson, 1982). Second, a prevalent ideology is that special programming should begin in middle childhood. Underlying this ideology are several beliefs: (a) Ability will have stabilized by the time children are about 8 or 9 years old; (b) psychoeducational testing is unreliable before middle childhood; and (c) early childhood and primary school educators are well equipped to meet individual needs within their classrooms.
Another powerful ideology is that matching advanced developmental characteristics with appropriate curricula puts unnecessary stress on young children. Parents and teachers who respond to childrenâs advanced capabilities by offering them opportunities to develop these abilities often are viewed with suspicion as being âpushy.â Advocacy for accelerated school placements may also be viewed as a desire to speed up development in an unhealthy fashion, with negative socialemotional consequences (Southern & Jones, 1991). Also influencing the availability of educational opportunity is the belief that gifted young children have the intellectual and social-emotional resources to get what they need from the educational system; in other words, they will âmake it on their own.â
There is little research support for the ideologies and systemic perspectives just described. Most of these powerful beliefs stem from anecdotal data. This is not to say that there may not be some children for whom one or more of the issues may be salient. There is always the imperative to consider each child individually, taking into account family, school, and community contexts. However, there also is a compelling imperative to examine these ideologies in light of relevant research.
Psychological Perspectives and the Nature of Advanced Development
Psychology offers important perspectives on the nature of advanced development, including explanations and descriptions of conceptual understanding and skills, different developmentalpathwaystogiftedness,asynchronyindevelopment, motivation, and social and emotional development. Precocity in development may be evident in infancy. Retrospective parental reports of unusually advanced development in infancy (e.g., Feldman, 1986) may be questioned for their reliability; however, observations and behavioral ratings of infants have been linked prospectively to intellectual and academic precocity (e.g., Gottfried, Gottfried, Bathurst, & Guerin, 1994; Louis & Lewis, 1992). Domain-specific abilities (e.g., linguistic or mathematical precocity) are apparent in very young children, and these abilities tend to be stable over time (Dale, Robinson, & Crain-Thoresen, 1995; Robinson,Abbott, Berninger, & Busse, 1996).
In addition to the strikingly advanced capabilities of young gifted children, some age-typical or even below-average capabilities may be evident. Gifted young children demonstrate considerable inter- and intra-individual variance, a phenomenon described as asynchronous development (Morelock, 1996). One of the ways in which their development may be more closely related to chronological than mental age is in their conceptual understanding of a domain. Gifted young children may bump up against a conceptual ceiling that is related to maturation (Fischer & Canfield, 1986; Porath, 1992).
For example, in coordinating plot elements in narrative or rendering perspective, abilities that are related to stage and structure of thought (Porath, 1996a, 1997), gifted children may be distinguished more by the complex use of the thought available to them than by exceptional developmental stage advancement. Gifted young children also may differ from each other in their developmental pathways.
Multiple Expressions of Giftedness
Each young gifted child has a distinct profile of abilities. In addition, there are multiple pathways to, and demonstrations of, giftedness in any one domain (Fischer, Knight, & Van Parys, 1993; Golomb, 1992; Porath, 1993, 2000; Robinson, 1993). Perhaps the best way to illustrate this is to introduce some young gifted children: Jessica, Holly, Sara, Sam, and Jill, five of the gifted children who participated in a series of studies on giftedness.
Six-year-old Jessica arrived to participate in a research project on narrative full of enthusiasm about her abilities to read and write: âI can read. Do you want me to read? I can write stories. Can Iwrite astory for you?â (Porath, 1986, p. 1). Jessica and the researcher settled on tape-recording a story to allow for production to keep pace with thinking. Jessica told âThe Leprechaunâs Gold,â a story richly representative of the fairy tale genre (see Porath, 1996, for the storyâs text). Jessica had a strong sense of story and a remarkable feel for language. Her vocabulary and syntax were mature, and she incorporated appropriate dialogue into her story.All of the aforementioned are advanced capabilities in a child of 6 years. Other 6-yearolds with whom Porath has worked have demonstrated their advanced verbal abilities in different ways: by playing with words (a story featuring a cat named Nip); through understanding the power of language (using rhymed couplets as humorous additions to a story); and by wanting to know all they can about words and their derivations.
Holly, aged 4.5 years, drew pictures in an enthusiastic, confident way with obvious mastery of different media. Her drawings of human figures were detailed and well proportioned. Most striking was her feel for composition, described by an art educator as entailing âsome rather sophisticated propositions regarding spatial organizationâ (p. 31). The simple formal harmony of the composition also was noted. Sara, age 6, showed her talent in a somewhat different way, producing elaborate drawings with colors chosen for contrast and detail (Porath, 1993). Sam, age 2, showed advanced social role-taking ability. While playing with the toy telephone in his preschool, Sam initiated a conversation with, âHello! I fine!â followed by adult-like speech inflections and twirling of the telephone cord. He engaged both a peer and his teacher by offering them the telephone and saying, âTalk to you?â (Porath, 2000, p. 203).
Jill, age 4, demonstrated social capability through her advanced interpersonal understanding, as elicited through responses to a picture story of a little girl who has lost her favorite book. Jill showed remarkable abilities to incorporate multiple emotions into her responses and empathize with the protagonist. Part of the discussion with Jill is reproduced below:
Researcher: How was the little girl feeling?
Jill: Surprised, mad, and sadâall three.
Researcher: Why?
Jill: Because her friends were near. Thatâs why sheâs mad; and she was sad and surprised because her book was lost. [Upon being asked what another character in the story would do, Jill replied that he would help].
Jill: Because he didnât want her to be sad and lost it again. You know why? Cause I lost my kitty . . . my favorite kitty, I would be feeling like that. (Porath, 2001, p. 20)
Motivation, Social-Emotional Development, and Giftedness
Young gifted children are notable for their motivation to learn (Gottfried et al., 1994; Robinson, 1993). Winner (1996) characterized this high intrinsic motivation as âa rage to masterâ (p. 3). Claire Golomb (1992) powerfully illustrated this rage to master in her research on an artistically gifted boy who, from the age of 2, explored topics by drawing numerous variations on a theme from every possible point of view. Similarly, at age 4 Wang Yani, an artistic prodigy, painted monkeys until she had exhausted the subject, often sustaining interest for incredibly long periods of time (Wang, 1987). Young gifted children may play an important role in creating environments that sustain them (Robinson, 1987).
In general, young gifted children are well adjusted socially and emotionally (Robinson & Noble, 1991). They tend to be socially mature, preferring older childrenâs company. However, young gifted childrenâs social-emotional development, while mature, still can be discrepant from their cognitive development. This asynchrony can interact with parental and teacher expectations of across-the-board maturity, with the possible result of denial of appropriate programming until the child âimprovesâ socially. Another conundrum is that young gifted children, faced with an insufficiently challenging curriculum, will âact their age,â again with the possible result that social behavior becomes the focus of educational efforts rather than appropriate curriculum (Keating, 1991).
Very highly gifted children are the exception to the general finding of healthy social-emotional adjustment among gifted children. Because these childrenâs capabilities are so unusual, they find it very difficult to find their place in the world (Hollingworth, 1942; Winner, 1996). When asked how he was the same as and different from other children, a highly gifted 6-year-old replied, âI like Raphael and Michelangelo, but as artists not as Ninja Turtlesâ (Porath, 1996b, p. 15). This child struggled to find his place, both academically and socially, from the time he entered school.
Educational Implications
Psychological perspectives on the development of gifted children have important implications for early childhood education. The findings on the early emergence and stability of exceptional capabilities strongly suggest that the provision of an optimal curricular match to these advanced abilities in early childhood settings is essential to nurture young childrenâs curiosity, motivation, and accomplishment (Keating, 1991; Robinson et al., 1996). Well-designed programs that honor the constructive nature of understanding sustain childrenâs excitement in learning and extend their achievements (Freeman, 2000; Robinson, Abbott, Berninger, Busse, & Mukhopadhyay, 1997). These programs also have the potential to support different developmental pathways.
The importance of considering the nature and structure of conceptual understanding in planning educational programs is underscored by work on expertise. The knowledge structures of experts are complex; instruction that has as its objective the nurturance of expertise incorporates meaningful conceptual material (Bereiter & Scardamalia, 1986). Instruction that fails to incorporate conceptual material can lead to cumulative deficits in achievement (Meichenbaum & Biemiller, 1998). This outcome was illustrated powerfully by Jeanne Bamberger (1982) in her study of gifted adolescent musicians. These young musicians went through a sort of midlife crisis when called on to learn formal theoretical musical concepts. Having experienced a largely skills-based approach to musical instruction since early childhood, they were unprepared for the demands to think more deeply about music and became frustrated and unmotivated.
Well-planned academic programs also support social and emotional development (Keating, 1991). Creative and flexible options are needed to address the diversity of development among young gifted children. Most particularly, highly gifted children need options that are sensitively matched to the needs implicit in development that includes abilities far ahead of even other gifted young children. A ânurturant resourceful environmentâ (Meichenbaum & Biemiller, 1998, p. 13) is necessary to support high motivation. Children need to be helped and supported to be interested (Bereiter & Scardamalia, 1986), even when they are highly intrinsically motivated. Intensive early support and education result in high levels of competence and satisfaction (Bloom, 1985).
Giftedness is apparent in early childhood. Its presence signals a need to step outside the confines of a lockstep approach to education in which age and grade seem inextricably joined. When faced with compelling advanced developmental needs, the predictive question of subsequent performance is not relevant; however, an optimal match between school program and abilities is. Keating (1991) suggested that the following question be posed: âIs there an appropriate match between the child and the program?â If the answer is negative, then we must ask what needs to be done to make the program appropriate.
Directions for Future Research
Important guiding principles for early educational experiences for gifted children are apparent in psychological research. These principles need to be translated into an applied researchprogramtoguidepractice.Weneedrichlydescriptive classroom-based and system-based examples of exemplary practice and studies of intervention effects. Coupled with a need to expand how we conceive of intelligent behavior (see, e.g., Ceci, 1996; Gardner, 1983) is the need for valid and reliable assessment tools that go beyond IQ. Basic research into cognitive processes and the nature of abilities across domains needs to continue. Longitudinal research that includes an applied dimension would strengthen our understanding of the nature and development of giftedness. The complexities of gifted abilities and achievements are recognized (Freeman, 2000), as is the dynamic relationship between young children and their environments (Smitsman, 2000). This intensive and challenging research agenda is essential to ensuring appropriate early childhood education opportunities for children who demonstrate exceptional potential.
Linguistic and Cultural Diversity in Early Childhood
Recent decades have witnessed an increasing number of immigrants to many, if not most, Western industrialized countries. This continuing upswing in immigration and demographic changes has resulted in a dramatic increase of students with diverse ethnic, cultural, and linguistic backgrounds in the school system and in ECE programs of many industrial societies, especially in NorthAmerica. Within existing societal contexts where family values, education equity, and human rights are discussed, contemporary education faces new understandings andchallengestomeettheneedsofculturallyandlinguistically diverse students who had been historically placed in contexts of social, economical, and educational vulnerability.
An important challenge in educating minority students has been how best to help them learn the majority language and culture without precluding them from developing and maintaining their native languages and cultures. In fact, decades of literature addressing best educational practices for minority children continue to stress the importance of developing home languages and cultural identity (Campos, 1995; Cummins, 1979, 1992; Hakuta, 1986; Paul & Jarvis, 1992). Such findings, however, have not exerted much influence on the practices in early childhood education (Bernhard, Lefebvre, Chud, & Lange, 1995; Kagan & Garcia, 1991; LaGrange, Clark, & Munroe, 1995; Soto, 1997). Research in this area has indicated that large numbers of early childhood settings have adopted at best superficial or token expressions of cultural diversity, such as presenting diverse holidays, foods, or customs. The issue of how to balance the competing interest between developing native and second languages during the early years has still relied ultimately on parental and individual struggles. A lack of responsiveness and efficacy for the diverse populations in many early childhood programs may be due partly to the limited understanding of cultural and language issues in the process of transition from the home culture to that of the school in minority children (Bernhard et al., 1991).
This brief discussion is an attempt to introduce some of the language and cultural issues raised regarding young immigrant children with a focus on the role and importance of the childrenâs home languages and cultures in ECE settings.
Second-Language Learning in the Preschool Years
Historically, researchers who have explored issues of secondlanguage learning and language maintenance in immigrant populations have been more concerned with school-age children than with preschool-aged children (Cummins, 1986; Lyon, 1996). In the past decade, however, researchers have become increasingly concerned about language learning of 2- to 6-year-old immigrant children due to the general increase in early education enrollment and growing minority populations, resulting in more and more immigrant children becoming exposed to a majority language during this early period. In addition, because younger children are presumed to be better language learners than older children, there has been a strong emphasis on immigrant childrenâs being fluent in the majority language prior to school entry. As a result, the goal of preparing immigrant children in the majority language has been pushed down to the preschool level (Kagan & Garcia, 1991; Prince & Lawrence, 1994; Wong Fillmore, 1991).
Within this context, researchers have become increasingly concerned about the consequences of emphasizing English to language-minority children during the preschool years. The major concern is that this could result in a subtractive bilingual experience; that is, the addition of the majority (second) language can cause a deterioration or erosion of the native (first) language. Thus, instead of English serving as enhancement to the childâs linguistic repertoire, it often serves as a replacement (Lambert & Taylor, 1983; Wong Fillmore, 1991; Wright, Taylor, & Macarthur, 2000). Such subtractive processes can be associated with considerable cognitive, emotional, and developmental risks (Cummins, 1991; Skutnabb-Kangas, 1981; Wong Fillmore, 1991; Wright & Taylor, 1995). The extent to which such language loss or subtractive bilingualism takes place during the preschool years and the role that early educational institutions play in language development of these children, however, have been matters of significant debate. We turn now to this debate.
The Debate on Early Schooling of Language Minority Children
Perhaps the two important and interrelated questions raised in this debate are these: When should children be exposed to the majority language in a school-like setting? Is there a threshold of native language skills that children should reach before the majority language is introduced? In which language should children be instructed in their early years?
A rapid acquisition of English in a school-like setting before competence in the native language has been achieved, combined with a corresponding lack of incentive to develop the native language, not only slow the native language development but also adversely lead to difficulties in acquisition of a second language (Cummins, 1986; Schiff-Myers, 1992). To explain this bilingual language delay, many have cited Cumminsâs (1984, 1986) hypotheses of threshold and developmental interdependence, which would suggest that a childâs second language competence is partly a function of the competence previously developed in the first language. The child must acquire and maintain a threshold level of proficiency, which include literacy skills as well as aural and oral skills, to avoid the subtractive effects of second language instruction. Therefore, if the first language proficiency is at a lower stage before the second language is introduced, it is more difficult to develop the second (Cummins, 1986; Lambert & Taylor, 1983; Skutnabb-Kangas, 1981; Soto, 1993).
The acquisition of a second language at the preschool stage can also lead to the loss of the home language (Cummins, 1991; Faulstich Orellana, 1994; Siren, 1991), which can, in turn, cause serious problems with communication and socialization within the families (Wong Fillmore, 1991). This does not mean that learning English, even in a limited way, should be a taboo for young minority children; however, as Wong Fillmore (1991, p. 345) stressed, âThe problem is timing, not English. The children have to learn English, but they should not be required to do so until their native languages are stable enough to handle the inevitable encounter with English and all it means.â
However, other researchers have recommended that language-minority children take advantage of an early start in introducing both languages (Baker & deKanter, 1983; Porter, 1991). Language skills require up to 7 years of practice to reach the levels necessary for academic learning, and learning English in an ECE program can support the natural and easy acquisition of a second language. Porter (1991), for example, argued that early immersion in a second language, preferably between ages 3 and 5, offers the greatest opportunity to learn native pronunciation and the highest level of literacy in that language. The earlier the children learn a second language, the more easily they appear to achieve high levels of fluency.
The matter then rests as a problem of deciding the appropriate time and manner of exposing preschool-aged children to a second-language setting to promote the childrenâs bilingual competence, academic success, and family relations. A missing component in this discussion is how this issue can affect parents, who have the ultimate decisions of when and how to balance the competing interest between the two languages. How, exactly, are immigrant parents of young children dealing with these issues?
In a study of immigrant parents in Sweden, Siren (1991) reported that although most parents supported the goal of bilingual development, there were differences of opinion among parents regarding when this goal of bilingualism should be achieved. According to Soto (1993), parents of academically high-achieving Puerto Rican immigrant children in kindergarten through second grade strongly believed in developing a foundation in Spanish first while gradually introducing English to their children. Parents of children who were lower achievers also valued Spanish, but they expressed a preference for simultaneous learning in both languages and were much more ambivalent about the developmental progression of first- and second-language learning. In her study of Korean immigrant families in Canada, Koh (2000) similarly found that parents who enrolled their children in bilingual preschools expressed a strong preference for their children building a strong foundation in Korean before learning English. Accordingly, they tended to provide exclusive Korean language environments at home. In contrast, Korean immigrants who enrolled their children in English-speaking preschools believed that children at an early age could acquire two languages naturally and actively supported this scenario by their choice of preschool setting and the nature of home literacy activities. Thus, a major question among immigrant parents of young children concerns the most appropriate balance between developing fluency in both their native language and the majority language. Parental attitudes influence language practices and interaction patterns at home and parentsâ selection of a particular preschool program.
There is general agreement among researchers that a childâs success in learning both the native language and the majority-culture language is more dependent on the nature of the home and preschool learning environments and less dependent on the childâs age per se (Arnberg, 1987; SchiffMyers, 1992). In other words, minority childrenâs secondlanguage learning in the early years is not always a problem if the home languages and literacy are valued and encouraged in the home, preschool, and community. Nevertheless, the appropriate balance between developing the native language and the majority language during the early school years is a critical issue about which language minority parents and ECE practitioners need to have more solid theoretical and empirical bases.
Another hotly debated issue surrounds the instructional use of the two languages in minority childrenâs early schooling (Garcia, 1993). At one end of this debate are proponents of native or bilingual language instruction who recommend learning of the native language prior to the introduction of an English curriculum (transitional or maintenance bilingual education). In this approach it is suggested that âcompetencies in the native language, particularly as related to academic learning, provide important cognitive and social bases for second-language and academic learningâ (Garcia, 1973, p. 379). The other end of this debate recommends immersion to the English curriculum from the very start of the studentsâ schooling experience with the minimal use of the native language (English immersion education). Perhaps the most familiar examples of this debate are results on bilingual or native-language ECE classrooms for Spanish speakers in the United States (see Bilingual Research Journal, 1992). However, this issue could extend to a wide variety of languageminority groups who have a choice of native or bilingual language instruction in their early years. Evaluation of immersion, native, or bilingual programs includes political and ideological as well as methodological and technical debates.
However, a number of researchers have increasingly noted a positive outcome in the language and cognitive development for the children attending effective bilingual or native language classrooms (Campos, 1995; Paul & Jarvis, 1992; Rodriguez, Diaz, Duran, & Espinosa, 1995).
The language of instruction is but one of many factors that can determine the outcomes of learning in a specific early education program. These other factors could include teacher training, classroom interactions, the nature of peer relations, the curriculum that is used, parent involvement, or program philosophies that contextualize the teaching and learning in the programs. Further study of language practices in the early schooling of language-minority children should focus on more than the language of instruction alone and should include such ecological factors of the programs. The issue of minority studentsâ early education should also be understood not only in the classroom or school but also in a broader societal context where the children are learning the native and majority language (Garcia, 1993; Wong Fillmore, 1991).
The Role of Home Culture in Early Childhood Education
In the past, the language and culture that minority families and children brought from their home into new environment were considered to be deficiencies (Collins, 1988). Deficiency theory attributed the academic failure of children from certain ethnic groups to culturally determined socialization practices in the home. Much recent and current research has helped to refute this cultural deficiency model and has begun to contribute to our understanding of cultural differences between the school and the minority students. The cultural differencesâor discontinuitiesâposition, supported by a number of well-documented ethnographic studies, suggests that a major source of childrenâs education failure lies in the culture clash between home and school (e.g., Delgado-Gaitan & Trueba, 1991; Garcia, 1988; Heath, 1983).
Others have pointed out that deficit-difference models have oversimplified the problem of minority achievement by generalizing and perhaps overemphasizing home-school discontinuities (Goldenberg, Reese, & Gallmore, 1992; Weisner, Gallimore, & Jordan, 1993). Although home-school discontinuities must be recognized and accommodated appropriately in the school, not all cultural discontinuities between minority families and school are negative (DelgadoGaitan & Truba, 1991; Schmidt, 1998; Volk, 1997; Weisner et al., 1993). Thus, it is necessary to look at some new and emerging data that suggest certain positive outcomes of some discontinuities. Weisner et al. (1993) gave an example of how educators identified cultural differences between school and Hawaiian families and then incorporated the familyâs teachinglearning style from the home culture into their classroom practices:
Sibcare is a ubiquitous aspect of Hawaiian child life; it reinforces habits of attending and orienting to siblings and other children rather than to adults. These habits can be generalized to the classroom by providing opportunities for peer teaching and learning. In a traditional classroom, these culture-based habits lead to problems; in classrooms using peer teaching, they produce better achievement. (p. 62)
Similarly, the Kamelahama Early Education Program (KEEP) in Hawaii showed that incorporating features of the home culture into reading lessons in primary classrooms resulted in an increase of the childrenâs reading achievement scores (Grant, 1995). Instead of mainstream classroom discussions of stories, discussions were adjusted to incorporate the Hawaiian storytelling tradition, which involves a high proportion of turn taking and cooperative production of responses (Au, 1997; Au & Carroll, 1997). Studies such as these showed that minority home cultures and values must be factored into childrenâs educational plans. Modifying old approaches and developing new conceptual insights and educational practices can help minority language children succeed in school.
The Socialization and Resocialization of Young Minority Children
Socialization is a means by which patterns of behavior, attitudes, values, and beliefs are transmitted to children from families, schools, peers, the media, and other influences (Garcia, 1993). During the early childhood years, family is essentially the major socialization source for a child. After the child enters early education programs, both the family and the school are primary socialization sources. The transition from home to early schooling marks a critical socialization period for all children, and perhaps more so for culturally and linguistically diverse children whose languages and ways of life are different from those of the mainstream (Kagan & Garcia, 1991; Villarruel, Imig, Kostelnik, 1995).
Families from diverse ethnic backgrounds may have different values, goals, and practices concerning what they consider important early experiences for children. These variations can reflect differences in their cultural belief systems about the status and role of children in society and their perceptions of how children learn (Goodnow, 1988). As an example, the traditional culture and socialization of northeast Asian countries are deeply rooted in Confucian philosophy, which strongly emphasizes interdependence from parents, group harmony, close family ties, and respect to adults. Children raised in this kind of cultural consciousness are not encouraged to initiate conversations with adults or to compete verbally with others publicly. These precise characteristics of socialization, however, are inconsistent with those of Western culture, which tend to emphasize the development of self-expression, autonomous choice, and independent individuals (Choi, Kim, & Choi, 1993). When the cultural climate of the home differs significantly from that of school and society at large, the child may be caught in the middle of home-school value conflict.
What happens if ECE programs of these children do not understand the values and child developmental goals behind cultural differences and seek to resocialize the children to a new set of values? Clearly, an appropriate function of education is socialization, and it is not unrealistic to expect that older children will accommodate such norms. However, asking very young children to be resocialized, just as formative socialization is taking place, unduly burdens them (Kagan & Garcia, 1991). The attempt to fit in to the socializationresocialization conundrum can result in confusion within the child and the rejection of the home culture and language. This in turn often creates a deep emotional gap between the child and the family and in some cases could lead to emotional or learning problems for the child (Delgado-Gaitan & Trueba, 1991; Kagan & Garcia, 1991). In fact, reports of Asian immigrants in North America have indicated a significant challenge in their childrenâs socialization and serious communication problems with their children at home (Kim & Choi, 1994; Lee & Lehmann, 1986; âThere is no Utopia,â 1999). Pettingill and Rohner (1985), who compared childrenâs perception of parenting among Korean, Korean Canadian, and Korean American samples, found that perceived parental control was correlated with high parental warmth and low neglect, and slightly with parental hostility and rejection in the Korean sample. On the other hand, for the Korean Canadian and Korean American children, parental control was perceived as low warmth and high hostility, neglect, and rejection. This study showed that parental controlâa positive feature of the parent-child relationship in Koreaâwas transformed into a negative feature for the children of Korean immigrants in North America.
Although there are always individual variations within the cultural groups, it is important to understand that the socialization characteristics of one cultural group are governed by its cultural values and beliefs and can be significantly different from those of another. Schools need to understand that minority familiesâ cross-cultural adjustments can be a difficult and complex process, so schools should provide learning environments that can mediate the adjustment and develop a healthy sense of self.
Closing Thoughts on Language, Culture, and Immigration
With so many children from diverse language and cultural groups in early childhood programs, it has become an increasingly important challenge to appreciate the significance of the familyâs language and culture and to move from appreciation to action on the part of early childhood professionals. A number of key issues regarding language, culture, and the education of young immigrant children must remain high priorities for both early childhood educators and researchers.
First, there is concern with the possibility of subtractive bilingualism, that is, losing proficiency in the childâs first language. Supporting the childâs native language at an early age has been strongly encouraged for the childrenâs academic success, bilingual proficiency, family relations, and a healthy sense of self. ECE programs for minority children must encourage and appreciate the language skills and cultural values that these children bring from their home, so that they neither directly nor indirectly stop the childrenâs native language and cultural development.
Parents of minority preschoolers are challenged to decide when and how to provide the relative balance between the native and second languages for their children, and they need more information and support. Studies of the impact of family culture on childrenâs learning have showed that identifying cultural differences between the school and the families, and incorporating the information into classroom practices, resulted in better academic achievement.
For the children of minority groups, parenting values and practices at home may be very different from what educators expect, and socialization experiences may be unique and complex as children often interact in multiple linguistic and cultural groups. Thus, ECE professionals are expected to look at children within the context of their family and culture. The potential conflicts and confusion that these families and their children might experience in their cross-cultural adjustment must be acknowledged, understood, and overcome.
Child Temperament and Early Childhood Education
I didnât get any information about temperament in my training. That community talk was when I first learned about Thomas and Chessâs study, you know, and about temperament. I didnât learn that in my early childhood training. Thatâs terrible, right? (âCorinne,â cited in Andersen & McDevitt, 2000, p. 14)
The early childhood special educator just quoted reported that she had to remove her 3-year-old son from a day care setting in which he was in constant conflict with teachers and peers. She was on a desperate search for caregivers who were knowledgeable and skilled in working with challenging behavior. She was also seeking emotional support and practical suggestions for herself from early intervention professionals. This parent had learned from medical and mental health specialists that her child was neurologically intact but that his intensity, slowness to adapt, negative mood, high activity level, and short attention span were aspects of his temperament that were colliding with the expectations of his parents and teachers. However, her attempts to impart this well-informed assessment of the basis of her sonâs behavior fell on deaf ears in her neighborhood day care center. The skepticism she encountered left her feeling judged and blamed, and her son was labeled âdisturbedâ and âtroublesome.â Yet she herself could not blame the teachers, whose training, like her own, had included so little attention to the extensive research that now exists on child temperament (Andersen & McDevitt, 2000). Although her experience in encountering ignorance of temperament is tragically commonly reported (Andersen, 1994), it is also true that ECE professionals have been among the most enthusiastic supporters of the concept. Many have acknowledged that the topic needs more attention in personnel preparation and as a variable that affects the functioning of children in group settings (Andersen, 1990; Soderman, 1985).
The History of Temperament Research
The idea of temperament has a long history, dating back at least as far Hippocrates (Rutter, 1982). However, after the hereditarian views of personality that held sway in the nineteenth century were rejected, by the middle of the twentieth century childrenâs traits were considered either to be all learned or to be projections of their parentsâ often-distorted perceptions. These psychodynamic and social-learning theories of childrenâs individual differences were first systematically challenged by the formulation and investigation of the construct of temperament by the American psychiatrists Stella Chess and Alexander Thomas. The attribution of all differences in children, including infants, to differences in parental (usually maternal) handling or to the projection of parental personality characteristics, ran counter to their own personal and professional experiences (Chess & Thomas, 1987). They knew that they could not refute these ideas on the basis of their opinions alone. The task called for a major research enterprise. Their New York Longitudinal Study (NYLS) was initiated in 1956 and followed 133 children from early infancy to adult life. This turned out to be a landmark study, and these two American psychiatrists paved the way for a veritable revolution in our understanding of childrenâs individuality and development. Their work was soon followed, and expanded upon, by other leading thinkers. The topic of temperament has consequently played a central role in the development of new interactionist theories of child development and in the formulation of etiological theories in child and adolescent psychiatry. It has been accompanied by intensive discussion of basic research and clinical questions, many of which continue to arise as childrenâs individuality is investigated today. It is therefore useful to consider first the central themes that have recurred as the concept of temperament has been put to the empirical test and as new conceptual issues have emerged. Following this, the implications and empirical investigation of temperament in early childhood education and day care will be discussed.
The Definition of Temperament and Temperament Traits
Chess and Thomas defined temperament as the behavioral style of a person, distinguishing this aspect from the personâs abilities and motivations (Chess & Thomas, 1987). They have also characterized it as âa non-motivational factor in the determination of behavioral patternsâ (Chess & Thomas, in Carey & McDevitt, 1989, p. 26). They clarified this idea by explaining that an individual responds to an internal or external stimulus through the meditating effects of temperament, along with other factors, such as past events, cognitive level, subjective feelings, and ideals. Others have characterized temperament as a mediating variable, representing an individualâs response patterns and manner of coping with stress and adversity (Rutter, 1994). Carey and McDevitt (1995) stated that although there is still no universal agreement on a definition, the general usage is in accord with the view of Thomas and Chessâs original concept of behavioral style. They noted an important distinction between temperament and cognitive factors:
Thomas and Chess explain it as the âhowâ of behavior as contrasted with the âwhatâ (abilities or developmental level) and the âwhyâ (motivations and behavioral adjustment). This conceptual and empirical separation of temperament and cognitive function has been demonstrated at various times in childhood including infancy (Plomin et al., 1990, 1993) and the elementary school years (Keogh, 1986; Martin, 1989, 1989b). (p. 10)
Although new methods are now available to investigators, particularly in molecular genetics and in direct studies of brain function, no new findings have overturned earlier views that temperament represents an innate attribute of a child and arises from a combination of genetic, biological, and environmental contributors.

Chess and Thomas identified the nine temperament traits listed in Table 13.1. Although the traits developed by Chess and Thomas have formed the basis of several temperament scales, which in turn have been utilized clinically as well as in vast numbers of individual studies, others have developed different formulations of the construct. Drawing on Carey and McDevittâs (1995) discussion of these alternatives, a brief summary is presented in Table 13.2.

The Measurement and the Functional Significance of Temperament
Although temperament is manifested in behavioral styles, researchers, clinicians, and parents are aware that similar behavior can have any number of nontemperamental causes. It is not surprising, then, that the assessment of specific behavior as related to temperament and the measurement of an individualâs temperamental profile have posed especially difficult problems for researchers and clinicians alike. Although some investigators have used psychophysiological instruments to support their claims about biological factors, most contemporary research has been conducted using paperand-pencil checklists of childrenâs behavioral tendencies as observed by parents, teachers, investigators, and clinicians. These scales are usually based on the nine dimensions developed by Chess and Thomas. These instruments are designed to tap childrenâs behavioral dispositions by asking parents and other observers questions about the childâs typical behavior in day-to-day situations. These scales, and modifications of them, have been validated on large numbers of children internationally (Carey & McDevitt, 1995). Currently, several scales are available that are appropriate for use by ECE personnel, often in combination with parent ratings and observations (McDevitt, in Andersen & McDevitt, 2000). These include a new questionnaire for caregivers and preschool teachers, the Teacher and Caregiver Temperament Inventory for Children (TACTIC), which measures temperament, attention, emotions, and conduct in 2- to 7-year-old children, and the Basic Behavioral Assessment Scale (BBAS) by Carey and McDevitt, which measures behavioral adjustment in 4- to 14year-old children in the areas of behavior, achievement, selfrelations, internal state, and coping (S. C. McDevitt, personal communication, November 2001).
Although there appears to be a growing consensus on many issues raised in the earlier years, more recent discussions of temperament continue to reflect earlier concerns about measurement. In response to Clarke-Stewart, Fitzpatrick, Allhausen & Goldberg (2000) presentation of a short and easy measure of infant temperament, Carey (2000) has argued for caution when hoping that there could be a brief and simple way to capture a complex phenomenon.
Although the work of Chess and Thomas and their followers was published as early as the mid-1960s, the findings on the predictive validity of childrenâs individual differences in emotional disposition and behavioral style did not win acceptance until the early 1980s. This was in spite of the fact that several independent lines of research had essentially replicated the early findings (Graham, Rutter, & George, 1973; Barron & Earls, 1984; Maziade, Caron, Cote, Boutin, & Thivierige, 1990). Fears of a deterministic view of human behavior may have played a role in the reluctance of developmentalists to accept the notion that biologically based differences exist in childrenâs emotional and behavioral dispositions. Carey and McDevitt (1995) suggested that in clinical circles another central reason for the slow acceptance of temperament concepts may lie in the fact that the risks associated with temperament are seen to lie not in the temperament itself but in the lack of goodness of fit between the childâs temperament and the expectations and values of the environment. Clinicians may have found it difficult to consider the question of fit because this notion is not in keeping with the habit of looking for problems within either the child or the environment. However, such a contextual approach has become not only acceptable, but fully in keeping with current theoretical understandings of the interplay between childrenâs dispositions, abilities, and interests and the complex factors that interact with their individuality at all levels of the ecology.
Despite the early resistance to the concept and despite the clinical worldâs slowness to adopt complex etiological models, accumulating evidence has left little room for doubt about the reality and importance of temperament. Research in temperament has now involved the efforts of hundreds of scientists in numerous areas, and the second wave of temperament research has adopted stringent methodological standards and extended its reach across cultures and into the genome, utilizing research methods ranging from ethnographic studies to the most recent technology such as brain scans and electrophysiological measurement. Clinician-researchers have been appraising the role of temperament across culture and socioeconomic differences, exploring the interaction between temperament and physical status, and behavioral-geneticists and neuroscientists continue to study the relative contribution of genes and the environment, including the environment of the womb. Twin and adoption studies have revealed that there is a substantial genetic contribution to temperamentâabout 50% on average. (Contrary to previous views, there is compelling evidence that temperament in infancy is not highly under genetic control and that genetic influences are stronger in the postinfancy years. In addition, temperament is not as stable or continuous as was once proposed.) Investigation of the role of maternal hormones, stress, prenatal infections, and perinatal stress has reminded developmentalists that environmental contributors to temperament can be biological (Carey & McDevitt, 1995, pp. 18â24).At the same time, interactions of parenting style, family stress factors, and social pressures continue to be seen as important contributors to outcome.
In the past two decades temperament has been shown to affect a wide range of areas of childrenâs functioning, no less so in the preschool and school years than in infancy. Current knowledge about the functional significance of temperament was recently summarized by Carey (1998, pp. 27) as cited by Andersen and McDevitt (2000) as
- fundamental part of the parent-child relationship;
- significant factor in patterns of growth and feeding;
- predisposition to prolonged crying in infancy;
- possible contributor to sleep problems;
- reason some children are hard to discipline;
- major risk factor for social behavior problems;
- substantial component of school performance;
- factor in physical conditions; one factor in recurrent pain;
- partial determinant of response to crises.
Although temperament has been shown to play an important role in many areas of development, perhaps the most significant area for application in early childhood education is the role of temperament in childrenâs psychological adjustment and educational achievement. It is useful, therefore, to review the major findings about this relationship. One of the most important and replicated findings of the early temperament research was the identification of specific clusters of traits that differentiated parentsâ experiences with children. Forty percent of the sample of children who were rated by their parents as adaptable, approaching, mildly active, mildly intense, and regular in biological rhythms were described by their middle-class North American parents as âeasy.â These children fitted well with the demands of family life and cultural expectations and were found to be at low risk for (but not immune to) the development of psychological and educational problems (Chess & Thomas, 1987). Ten percent of the sample were, by contrast, described as âdifficult.â These children were low in adaptability, predominantly negative in mood, irregular, withdrawn in new situations, and intense in reactivity. A third group of children, comprising 15% of the sample, were mildly intense in response, mildly negative in mood, and slow to adapt to new situations. This group was described as âslow-to-warm-up.â
The most important finding of the NYLS was the discovery that âdifficultâ children were at significantly higher risk of developing problems in behavioral and emotional adjustment. However, adjustment problems were not inevitable. Chess and Thomas invoked the concept of goodness of fit to describe a compatible relationship between parental expectations and a childâs temperament; when the fit was poor, the child experienced excessive stress and developed reactive behavioral and emotional problems. The finding that temperamentally difficult children were at higher risk of developing psychological problems has been replicated in subsequent research (Earls & Jung, 1987; Graham et al., 1973; Maziade, Cote, Bernier, Boutin, & Thivierge, 1989; Maziade et al., 1990). However, fit has been shown to be context dependent. Consequently, Carey and McDevitt (1989) proposed the term temperament risk factors:
Temperament risk factors are any temperament characteristic predisposing a child to a poor fit (incompatible relationship) with his or her environment, to excessive interactional stress and conflict with his or her caretakers, and to secondary problems in the childâs physical health, development and behavior. These characteristics are usually perceived as hard to manage, but may not be. The outcome depends on the strength and durability of the characteristics and the environmental stresses and supports. (p. 195)
Temperament Research in Early Childhood Education
Despite the consensus that now exists about the importance of the role of childrenâs individual differences in temperament as a significant risk factor in specific contexts, applications of this knowledge in all levels of early childhood education have been few (Andersen, 1990). In 1995 Carey and McDevitt published an extensive review of the extant studies of temperament. In their chapter on day care, they decried the failure of the prevention, intervention, and caregiving communities to apply this important knowledge. In their chapters on infants and young children, they noted that âbehavioral investigation in the area [of child day care] has almost completely ignored the impact of childrenâs temperament on the experienceâ (p. 83). They summarized the little research there was at the time. However, what little research there is suggests that further investigation is likely to be fruitful (Anderson-Goetz & Worobey, 1984; Field & Greenberg, 1982; Keogh & Burstein, 1988; Palison, 1986).
Among the most important preliminary findings cited by Carey and McDevitt (1995) was the (counterintuitive to some) discovery that mothers of children with difficult temperaments were less likely to go back to work and to place their children in day care, although the authors commented that this study was conducted in the late 1950s and early 1960s, when âthe pressure to seek employment outside the home might not have been felt quite as strongly by middleclass mothers as it is todayâ (p. 85). More recently, Canadian researchers McKim, Cramer, Stuart, and OâConnor (1999) examined the family and child factors associated with child care decisions and found that mothers who preferred to stay home were more depressed and that their children were more likely to experience unstable care than were those who were working and wanted to work. The age at which the child entered day care and aspects of the care such as quality were not related to attachment measures. However, infants with difficult temperaments were at higher risk of being rated as insecurely attached. Attending day care appeared to reduce the strength of this relationship. This study, though preliminary, lends some support to the notion that enrolling a difficult infant in day care might have beneficial effects for the infant and possibly also for depressed mothers.
Another important question is how childrenâs individual temperaments affect their socioemotional adjustment to, and functioning in, early childhood group environments. In an early study of 2.5- to 3.5-year-old children entering day care, Billman and McDevitt (1980) obtained mothersâ and observersâ temperament ratings on a sample of 78 predominantly White middle-class children ranging in age from 34 to 64 months and attending two nursery schools in a small Midwestern American community. There were 40 girls and 38 boys in the study. The researchers set out to clarify the relationship between home-rated temperament and social behavior at nursery school, to investigate any relationship between difficult temperament and peer interaction, and to assess the consistency of temperament ratings made by different raters in different settings. Parental ratings of the temperament characteristic of low approach predicted teacher evaluations of slower adjustment. It is interesting to note that the âdifficult childâ cluster of traits emerged as a significant predictor of peer interactions. Although convergence between the two temperament ratings was moderate (.18â.46), correlations were statistically significant for all dimensions except mood. Activity level, approach-withdrawal, and sensory threshold were significantly related to peer interaction. Very active children were both more sociable than inactive children and more often involved in conflictual interactions. More rhythmic children spoke more to peers and got on with their tasks more effectively. When the children were assigned to the temperament clusters identified by Chess and Thomas, the difficult child was found to engage in more wrestling, hitting, jumping, pushing, and beating.
More recently, Harden et al. (2000) used Bronfenbrennerâs ecological framework to explore externalizing behavior problems among children enrolled in a suburban Head Start program and compared them with a subgroup of children with behavior problems in the clinical or borderline range. Childrenâs externalizing behavior was positively associated with internalizing behavior, parent psychological adjustment, child temperament, family environment, and exposure to community violence.
Yen and Ispa (2000) tested the hypothesis that curriculum type (Montessori and constructivist) moderates the impact of temperament (specifically, activity level and attention span and persistence) on the classroom behavior of 3- to 5-yearold children. A near-significant trend suggested that temperamentally active boys were more likely to be perceived by their teachers as having behavior problems if they were enrolled in Montessori programs than if they were enrolled in constructivist programs. An interesting finding was that attention span and persistence did not have any effect on the impact of the type of curriculum on childrenâs behavior. This is one of the few studies that explores the important question of the goodness of fit between specific types of curriculum and childrenâs temperament.
Stansbury and Harris (2000) conducted a study to ascertain whether standardized peer entry paradigm would produce stress responses in 38 3-year-olds and 25 4-year-olds and how such stress responses might correlate with temperament, approaches to peers, and peer competence as perceived by the children themselves. Four-year-olds were significantly less avoidant and were rated higher on the temperament trait of âapproach.â They showed larger Hypothalamic-PituitaryAdrenal Cortical Axis (HPA) stress responses to new peers, and the disparity between self-reported peer competence and behavior in the peer-entry situation was associated with greater stress responses on a physiological measure. The researchers stressed the importance of examining discrepancies between self-perception and action in research on stress.
The need for the education of early childhood professionals about temperament was acknowledged in a study by Franyo and Hyson (1999) of the effectiveness of temperament training of early childhood caregivers. This study provided information about caregiversâ preexisting knowledge of temperament concepts and investigated the effect of educational workshops about temperament concepts. The findings were that without training, caregivers had heard about temperament but knew little about the specifics of the empirical findings. Without training, caregivers did not appear to have many ideas about how to achieve a goodness of fit by using behavioral management techniques, although this was an area in which they showed special interest. Encouragingly, caregivers were very accepting of the concept of temperament, and training sessions were effective in increasing their knowledge about temperament concepts. However, the investigators reported that there was no statistically significant evidence that the training was effective in improving the caregiversâ acceptance of childrenâs behaviors and feelings.
Implications for Further Research
Although these new efforts are encouraging, there continues to be a dearth of empirical research on the role of child temperament in early childhood group settings. Much more work remains to be done. There are many areas of child temperament research that have direct implications for early childhood education. The first of these is the direct education of caregivers and teachers about the origins and significance of individual differences in childrenâs behavioral styles so as to avoid misattributing all these differences to central nervous system dysfunction or parenting. Second, research is demonstrating that temperament has an impact on the way children experience, and affect, their environments. Active children are often criticized by staff for their restlessness when a programmatic modification may be in their interests. Children who need time to adapt to new situations require teachers who have the patience to help them overcome their period of hesitation (Soderman, 1985). There is particular concern about children whose temperaments challenge and frustrate caregivers and about peers who may be the recipients of rejection or neglect, leading to the development of secondary problems. More work is needed in formulating effective training programs that will bring about both significant attitudinal and behavior management changes in caregivers. This is particularly important in view of the fact that families of children with challenging temperament-related issues may seek assistance from ECE staff. Because there is often an overlap between this group of children and those diagnosed at school age with attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder or oppositional defiant disorder, early intervention agencies should recognize that these children are at very high risk when the fit is poor and should be eligible for services under such circumstances (Andersen & McDevitt, 2000).
No less important than applying research about temperament risk factors is an appreciation of the fact that temperament traits can serve also as assets (Carey & McDevitt, 1995). Researchers would do well to explore how specific temperament traits may benefit children in their interactions with adult caregivers, with peers, and with the curriculum of ECE programs, and practitioners need to avoid automatically associating the word âtemperamentâ with risk factors. Carey and McDevitt stated the key questions for the conduct of research on temperament in day care this way:
What kinds of children do better in what kinds of care arrangements? Does a childâs temperament influence his or her behavior with peers and child care professionals in the day care situation in the same ways as in the home? How can we deliver day care that best meets the needs of parents and children? What happens when there is a poor fit between the childâs characteristics and the handling provided by the day care facility? How is this dissonance best detected? Will the management principles that seem to work for parents in the home also prove effective in the day care setting? What do day care workers generally know about important mental health matters, including temperament differences? Where do they get such information, and how successfully do they apply it? What is the best way to augment such knowledge? What benefits can be demonstrated for its use? What are the consequences of day care workers not having this knowledge? For aggressive or non-compliant behavior, what management strategies beside time-out are suitable? How can parents and day care workers best collaborate for the well-being of the child? (Carey & McDevitt, 1995, p. 90)
Closing Thoughts on Child Temperament
The field of early childhood education has made great strides and, indeed, has demonstrated leadership in showing respect to cultural diversity among young children and their families and in serving as teachers of, and advocates for, young children with disabilities and social risk factors. A burgeoning body of empirical studies in a wide range of fields has led to a consensus that child temperament exists, can be reliably measured, and is an important and universal individual difference among infants and young children that can serve as an asset or create risk. Respecting diversity among young children must now be extended to respecting this diversity in behavioral style. Early childhood researchers and practitioners will undoubtedly rise to the challenge of the new millennium by absorbing and applying the substantial knowledge that has been accumulated about temperament in the past four decades. We can look forward to seeing further attention to this important variable in empirical studies, in personnel preparation, and in curricula and programming.
Programs and Quality in Early Childhood Education
Compensatory programs and early childhood special education.
The title of âfather of the kindergartenâ was given to Friedrich Froebel. If Froebel is granted this acknowledgement, then the title of mother of early childhood special education (ECSE) should be granted to Maria Montessori (1870â1952). Indeed, Montessori anticipated the broad sweep that the field would make across the spectrum of childhood experiences and needs. In 1901 she became the director of the âorthophrenicâ school associated with the University of Rome, a former asylum for city children, most of whom were likely intellectually disabled or autistic. She mounted a campaign to reform the system that had previously confined these children to a life of isolation and neglect. Montessori understood the need they had for purpose in life, for stimulation and for achieving a sense of self-worth. Anticipating the modern ethos, Montessori emphasized the importance of showing respect to all children and of teaching them self-care skills as well as to observe and respect the world around them. Also foreshadowing current trends, Montessori took an interest in children who were at risk because they were poor.
An Overview of the History of Early Childhood Special Education
In 1907 Montessori began an experimental school in the poorest part of Rome to demonstrate that her educational methods could be effective with inner-city preschoolers who were not disabled. She also anticipated the contemporary approach to the field of ECSE by applying herself to a rigorous analysis of research on the education of the mentally handicapped, focusing on the work of two French doctors, Jean Itard and Edouard Seguin. Itardâs work with the Wild Boy of Aveyron brought into focus another constant theme in contemporary ECSE, the long-standing question of the relative weights of nature and nurture in childrenâs development. Seguinâs influence was to encourage Montessori in her already-strong belief in making education systematic and specific. Although many consider Seguin to be the father of special education, it is an irony of history that in North America Montessoriâs influence was not felt in the education of young children with special needs as much as it was in the development of Montessori nursery schools for normal children of middle- and upper-middle-class parents.
Indeed, in most Western countries the early education of young children at risk for compromised development was a neglected field until shortly after World War II. Although each modern state has followed its own path in developing and expanding services for children at risk, the United States is unique in mandating these servicesâa mandate that has driven academic research as much as it has program delivery. As a result, much research on the education of young children at risk has been conducted in the United States, and it is therefore the American experience in the field that followed federal legislation that will be reflected, for the most part, in this section.
As was described in the opening section of this research paper, the theme of nature versus nurture was present in the earliest years of the field of early childhood education. The 1961 publication of McVicker Huntâs Intelligence and Experience fueled a renewed emphasis on the role of environmental experience in cognitive development. In 1965 the U.S. federal government initiated the first Head Start program, a summer initiative to bring Americaâs poorest children into programs in which they would receive stimulation and nutrition and that would involve their parents in a broad-sweeping and early attempt to provide compensation for the known detrimental effects of poverty and social disadvantage on childrenâs development. These early short-term programs were followed by programs that were more intensive and broader in conceptualization. Consequently, by 1972 a mandated quota of 10% of children with disabilities in Head Start programs was established. And although the Education for All Handicapped Children Act (PL 94-142), passed in 1972, did not cover children from birth to 3 years of age, in time services for preschool-aged children were mandated, and standards and incentives for providing services to infants and toddlers were included. One outcome of international significance in the American legislation and experience has been the emergence of a unified discipline that subsumed all the early childhood risk factors under its umbrella: ECSE. This new field was heralded by the formation of the Division for Early Childhood as a subsection of the Council for Exceptional Children in 1973 (McCollum, 2000).
Early childhood special education is the branch of the field of early childhood education that addresses the diverse educational needs of children with disabilities and developmental delays. The field has also come to include children whose developmental risk was at first seen to arise from the disadvantage of being raised in certain family and sociocultural contexts. Although the term compensatory education has been used to describe the aim of programs attempting to make up for presumed deficits in childrenâs environments, many theorists now regard it as unreasonable to think in terms of a dichotomy between children with endogenous difficulties and those with problems arising from environmental factors. It has been known for some time now that many risk factors arising from social conditions, such as maternal ingestion of alcohol, are translated into biological problems in children. In addition, many genetic risk factors are triggered only in the presence of certain environmental conditions. Recent neurobiological research has confirmed the potentially devastating impact of social stresses on young childrenâs neurodevelopment. Recent medical research has also shown that the technologies that are used to sustain survival of very high risk infants may create additional disabilities in those children.
Multidisciplinary Perspectives on Early Childhood Special Education
The complexity of the difficulties that face contemporary young children is matched by the need for multidisciplinary academic inquiry informed by many specialties and with a knowledge base of evidence-based practices that are reproducible in a field that continues to be marginalized and underfunded. One consequence of the scope and diversity of needs in the field of ECSE has been a major effort, in past decades, in the development of a professional identity. McCollum (2000) discussed how, between 1976 and 1985, several related movements were influential in shaping the nature of ECSE. The development of empirically based systematic instruction, furthered by academic investigation based on current learning theories, came first. Second, attention to preventing school failure became a mandate of the states, and the beginnings of a relationship between early childhood education and ECSE were forged as attempts were made to identify those young children who might be in need of early intervention. Third, increasing attention was paid to the noninstructional roles of teachers as ecological theory began to influence the way that interventionists understood the importance of the social context of development. Finally, younger and younger children began to receive attention and intervention programs because infants and toddlers grew rapidly.
The impact of these diverse influences challenged the assumption that this new field was a branch of special education alone. As the influence of other specialties was brought to bear on the questions of how best to help young children whose development might not proceed well, the new field was increasingly being referred to not only in educational terms. In keeping with paradigm shifts that occurred in early childhood education in general, ecological perspectives transformed the way in which relationships between culture, society, family, and the individual child were construed. In addition, the vocabulary of health and human services became recognizable in the discourse of the field, which became more broadly known as early intervention. This term, which was formerly used exclusively to refer to a point of time in a clinical intervention, had now become a separate area of study and of practice. As Bailey (2000) has commented, in the past three decades the field of early intervention has grown rapidly from its inception to its current state of maturity.
As diverse as its origins were, it is to the credit of its leading thinkers that the field of early intervention has reached a remarkable consensus about its goals, methods, values, and achievements. This consensus, sharpened by the presence of self-critical dialogue within its ranks, is well demonstrated by the publication of two recent special issues of Topics in Early Childhood Special Education (TECSE) that present a compelling picture of the field as it stands today: Early Childhood Special Education in a New Century: Voices from the Past, Visions for Our Future, Parts 1 and 2. These papers provide rich and often personal historical accounts of issues as far-ranging as personnel preparation, remedial teaching, inclusion, new theoretical understandings, child care, social conditions, the role of governments in mandating services, methodological issues, interdisciplinary relations, and interventions for specific disabilities. Only some of the major themes of these special issues will receive attention here: (a) inclusion; (b) new theoretical and empirical influences, including a clearer conceptualization by Dunst of familycentered intervention as evidence-based practice; (c) lessons learned from the past; (d) current issues in personnel preparation; and (e) the impact of legislation on services.
The Principle and Practice of Inclusion
Zigler and Styfco (2000) pointed out that Head Start programs were deliberately segregationist at their conception. The idea was to provide special and separate programs for young children from disadvantaged environments. Although this practice effectively cut them off from opportunities to learn in the company of peers from wealthier backgrounds, at the time it was considered that these children would not otherwise attend community nursery schools. Rather, the motivation was to offer them enriching opportunities, including nutritional and health care, that could not be provided by their homes. Likewise, programming for young children with disabilities was equally separate because it was thought that community preschool teachers did not know how to teach them. The inclusion of young children with disabilities in regular preschool and day care programs began slowly, and with much resistance. It is of interest that inclusion is less often justified on empirical grounds than defended in terms of a philosophical position regarding the rights of children with disabilities to experience the same privileges as those without. The emergence of day care into the field of early childhood education added a new dimension to these issues as educational questions became intertwined with questions about custodial care and the needs of working parents.
Although there have been many separate rationales invoked from time to time as policy makers and trainers have sought to persuade communities to move toward the adoption of inclusive practices, Bricker (2000) reminded us that the movement was originally driven by a clearly formulated developmental integration approach, which emerged from examining the detrimental effects of institutionalization on young children. She commented that this was by no means a well-accepted notion at the time. Her account of the gradual acceptance of the principle of inclusion demonstrates how quickly a questioned idea becomes its opposite: âPerhaps the biggest change has been the shift in perspective of inclusion from an idea about how to improve services to young children with disabilities to a human ârightâ that has become closely tied to important movements focused on social equalityâ (p. 17). She acknowledged that inclusion is still hotly debated despite an apparent consensus about its desirability, as evidenced by position papers put forth by organizations such as the Division of Early Childhood of the Council for Exceptional Children. Bricker herself stated that
the continuing debate over inclusion will do well to focus on the reality of current parental choices when facing placement options for their children. For many parents, the debate raging at the philosophical level may be very different from the realities they face when deciding on a placement for their child. (p. 17)
On a more optimistic note, Odom (2000) argued that
thirty years of research and practice have produced a knowledge base that informs policy and practice. . . . [N]ow, more than any time in the past, we have a greater awareness of the type of support professionals can provide to create productive learning environments for children with and without disabilities and inclusive settings. With political will, local leadership, willing parents, and committed teachers, most young children with disabilities can benefit from inclusive settings. (p. 25)
It is noteworthy that Odom did not use the rather absolute statementsâthat inclusion benefits all childrenâthat have entered the position papers put out by both professional and advocacy organizations.
The issues that surround the implementation of inclusion and the emergence in the next decades of large-scale research projects bring us to consider the strengths and struggles of the endeavor to subject early intervention programs to rigorous inquiry. The Head Start experience demonstrates how essential it is to consider how laudable programs can fail in the details of their implementation when errors are made early in their planning. Zigler and Styfco (2000) acknowledged the extent of the problems that surrounded Head Start from its inception: First was an exclusionary ethos, in which economically disadvantaged children were served separately from their peers from wealthier homes. This segregation extended to excluding those who were disabled until recent times. Second, the lack of a broad unitary goal in favor of a set of discrete objectives led to a misconception of Head Start as a cognitive enrichment program and a decreased emphasis on its original interest in social competence. The third problem was that programming was based on the questionable construct of school readiness. Finally, and perhaps most seriously, was lack of attention to quality. The first of these problems and the difficulties surrounding solutions to it were discussed earlier. The likelihood that topics of school readiness, social competence outcomes, and quality of programming will continue to be pressing issues in the field is acknowledged briefly here.
As researchers look at outcomes in diverse populations, they will be challenged to define common goals that should receive emphasis in the education of all at-risk young children. It is therefore of significance that the special issues of TECSE that were published in the summer of 2000 highlighted two interrelated themes: social competence and language development. In examining the question of social competence, Strain and Hoyson (2000) used research on social-skills intervention with children with autism to illustrate how increasingly sophisticated theoretical formulations of essential research-to-practice questions have produced gains in our ability to engender ârapid, occasionally sustainable, and socially meaningful changes in childrenâs social behaviorâ (p. 116). They identified four basic assumptions that historically have been used to determine the targets of intervention: (a) the notion that the difficulty with communication is intrinsic to the individual; (b) the notion that the individual has acquired the difficulty as a result of a long-standing interactional history; (c) the notion that dyadic interactions between children sustain the problem; and (d) the notion that the entire ecology of the child with special needs is responsible for altering its norms and the manner in which it structures opportunities for social interaction.
Arguing that none of these assumptions is sufficient to produce effective intervention, they described how a fifth assumption has led to a comprehensive research-to-practice model for children with autism in the past two decades: the LEAP program. The positive results of LEAP are presented to illustrate the potential of both short- and long-term positive benefits of interventions that are individualized and datadriven, have a generalization (transfer of skills) focus, maximize learning opportunities, and add a focus on family skills to the preschool intervention. These results identify the intensity of the intervention as a critical variable in yielding sustainable, long-term outcomes. One of their conclusions will likely be absorbed as the next generation of researchers attempts to achieve similar gains with other populations of children at risk:
The social interaction challenges presented by young children with special needs represent a complex, multivariate universe. That multivariate universe demands complex, longitudinal, intensive approachesâapproaches holding greater promise for children and families but also greater challenges to our ways of designing, executing, and describing credible studies and longitudinal data sets. These are challenges worthy of all eager pioneers with a visionary spirit. (Strain & Hoyson, 2000, p. 121)
A complementary theme that emerges from Hartâs (2000) review of studies observing young children is the nature of the language interactions between children and their parents. As the field shifts from theory to data and back again, there is a constant return to childrenâs ecologies as the source of new understanding about how they learn. In reflecting on how much has been learned from observing the way children learn to express themselves in their conversations with their parents, Hart commented on the need for more information on the nature and extent of conversation between teachers and young children and between children and their peers in group environments. She discussed the challenges of group education in enhancing the ability of young children to express themselves:
Some teachers, like some parents, need more information concerning what children should be learning through play and how to arrange environments, materials, and activities that prompt and facilitate talking. Research is needed to add to interventions the power of engagement (Risley, 1977) and to design lowdemand environments (Wasik, 1970) that encourage children to talk when the only object is conversation. (p. 31)
New Theoretical Influences and Their Implications
The themes of social skills and communicative competence are not new to the field, but many argue that they still remain underemphasized in programming. Perhaps two of the most central, truly revolutionary theoretical and empirical influences facing the field in the next millennium are those emerging from brain research and those supporting the use of family-centered practices. New brain research is currently the political driving force behind many fresh initiatives for young children in North America (Greenspan, 1997; Shore 1997). It is of interest that despite its great press appeal, this paradigm is not widely heralded in the summer 2000 issues of TECSE as either the rationale for or the solution to the most pressing problems facing young children today. In fact, it is telling that Zigler and Styfco (2000) appear to be urging some caution in this regard as they reflect on the wildly exaggerated hopes that fueled the original Head Start programs: âIndeed, the current bandwagon that infant brains need constant stimulation for superior neural wiring and growth is the environmental mystique with a biological twist. . . . [T]he moral is that there is no quick fix for poverty and no magical treatment that will turn us all into Nobel laureates or Rhodes scholarsâ (p. 68).
Models stressing the importance of social processes as conceptualized by Vygotsky (1978) and of social contexts as conceptualized by Bronfenbrenner (1979) and his followers continue to hold sway. In his review and in anticipation of the âthird generationâ of research in the field, Dunst (2000) pointed to the intersection between childrenâs learning opportunities and parental supports as the pathway to instructional practices that are most likely to be development enhancing and reminded us that the influence of social support variables on parentsâ contingent responses to children continues to draw empirical support. He calls for further research on natural learning opportunities provided by family and community life, reminding us that interventionists often have to more learn from families than they have to teach them.
Dunstâs (2000) social support paradigm links child developmental outcomes clearly to family-centered interventions. He pointed out that evidence-based, family-centered help giving is becoming more highly specified and can be divided into two central elements: relational and participatory. âRelational practices are a necessary condition for effective practitioner/ family transactions. . . . [T]hey are not sufficient for either strengthening family competence or promoting new capabilities. The latter has been found to be the case only when the family is an active participant in achieving desired outcomesâ (pp. 100â101). Dunst acknowledged that his own interest in family-centered practices has become increasingly conceptual and empirical rather than philosophical: âI believe that a philosophically-based, family-centered approach is likely to run its course as a fad and is open to all kinds of criticism, as has recently occurredâ (p. 97). He anticipated the continuance of third-generation research that establishes and describes the evidence that supports specific early family-centered intervention practices shaped by an evidence-based social support paradigm. As the field absorbs the impact of the hard data, practitioners will be increasingly asked to demonstrate, rather than discuss, the specific skills that emerge from this knowledge.
Current Issues in Personnel Preparation
McCollum (2000) stated that âbetween the late 1960s and the present, personnel standards and preparation opportunities have grown from no state certification in ECSE and no targeted preparation programs to widespread certification and many pre-service programsâ (p. 80). By 1996 a set of published personnel standards were adopted by the Division of Early Childhood (DEC) and the NAEYC, and most states had some sort of certification for teachers of young children with disabilities. Credentialing programs had been set in place in many states, and ECSE programs at the university level became widely available. It is noteworthy that these events appear to be related directly to the United States legislated mandate described earlier, as there has not been a parallel growth in personnel preparation in countries such as Canada, where university-based ECSE courses are still something of a rarity (Andersen, 1999). However, even in the United States, personnel preparation has not kept pace with the expansion of services. This situation has led contemporary commentators to call for a renewed emphasis on this topic and for new training models that take account of important systemic changes. McConnell (2000) highlighted two such changes that she believed should receive special attention: The first is how both the content and the context of personnel preparation for those working at the preschool and primary levels should be altered given the inclusion of young children with disabilities in community settings. This new context calls for a revised understanding of the relationship of the early childhood educator to the early childhood special educator. The second change regards the delivery of services to infants and toddlers in the United States: The fee-for-service structure is driving a fragmented approach that is not in the interests of children and families. Regarding the latter, McConnell stated that few parents or practitioners are fully aware of the danger that this practice poses to the ability to provide integrated, family-centered interventions. In turn, this may lead to the loss of ECSE generalists and to their replacement by a service coordinator who lacks a solid foundation in child development. She called upon the field to protect children by more clearly defining its own identity and importance for their development.
Winton (2000) argued that the field has currently failed to implement what research has demonstrated needs to be done toimprovetheoutcomesforallyoungchildren.Shepositsthat the development of learning communities of multiple partners at multiple levels will help to create the shared vision and commitment from the community that will help to bridge this research-practice gap. She claims that this in itself is an empirically supportedapproach to personnel preparation, in spite of the very real barriers to its implementation. She suggested a series of small stepping-stones and identified some successful models as the pathway to this important destination.
The Impact of Legislation
Bailey (2000) stated that the field of ECSE in the United States would never have arisen in this fashion without the commitment of the U.S. government. He anticipated that the role of the federal government will now shift from its emphasis on legislated mandates to a more facilitative role, with more decision-making power being left in the hands of the states. He asserted that a strong federal presence will continue to be needed and anticipated the funding of large research projects of nationally representative populations.
Most contributors to these special summer 2000 issues of TECSE see legislated mandates as a positive measure but not without problems. Bruder (2000), in arguing that the field has not successfully fulfilled a commitment to the familycentered practices it espouses, suggested that the programmatic requirements under some sections of the legislation are complex and require more skills and knowledge of the theory than are currently held by state and local administrators. (This problem is complicated by the use of finance models made up of billable services created on a child-centered rehabilitation model rather than a family-centered approach.) She also saw categorical and discipline-specific funding streams as creating barriers to the implementation of truly familycentered services. Odom (2000), too, saw similar bureaucratic barriers to the effective implementation of inclusive practices, some of which arise from administratorsâ misconception that inclusive programs cost more than traditional special education programs. Other barriers include policies over the manner in which funds are used: In some states money can be spent on tuition to permit a child with disabilities to spend some of his or her day in a community-based program; in other states this is not permissible. The allocation of special education teachers to nonspecialized settings is prohibited in some programs because the specialized teachers are paid out of special education funds. He regarded the flexibility with which administrators handle budgets as a key factor in overcoming these bureaucratic obstacles.
Although her discussion of the impact of federal legislation on personnel preparation is largely positive, McCollum (2000) stated that âthe different paths taken by those interested in birth to 2 and in ages 3 to 8 after the passage of PL99457 was an unfortunate outcome of the new legislationâ (p. 85). When states were permitted to use different agencies and different personnel requirements for the two age groups, a dual system of service and personnel preparation was enabled that created identity problems in the field. The field of ECSE was redefining its identity in relation to early childhood education and special education, with services delivered in educational settings; but with the younger group of infants and toddlers, professional identity was being reconceptualized through comparisons to noneducation disciplines. Indeed, the publications or organizations such as Zero to Three clearly demonstrate the influence of fields such as social work. As well some infant-parent intervention programs have adopted psychotherapeutic models from infant psychiatry. Corresponding with this divergence from the main discipline of ECSE, a number of systemic changes were occurring, complicating the question of personnel roles even further. In the same series, Smith (2000) discussed the need to shift efforts from the federal to the local level so that widespread community support, including fiscal support, will sustain and enhance the achievements begun with federal initiatives and legislation. Smith (2000) argues that communities need to be willing to invest tax dollars and to rethink social policy so that parents can spend more time at home. She also calls for greater respect for early childhood professionals and enhancement of the work environment and salaries so that the field will attract better qualified individuals.
Bailey (2000) noted that the role of the U.S. federal government was shifting from its emphasis on legislated mandates to a more facilitative role, with more decision-making power being left in the hands of the states. He asserted that a strong federal presence will continue to be needed and anticipated the funding of large research projects of nationally representative populations. Perhaps this is where the international field of early childhood education will feel the impact the mostâas new research projects of sufficient scale and quality emerge that further our understanding of the impact of specific educational practices with targeted but representative groups of young children.
If the next decade will bring new large-scale studies with results that convince policy makers, there will continue to be a major challenge in bringing that knowledge to the field. Several authors of the special issue decried the researchpractice gap in ECSE. Bruder (2000), in particular, focused on the failure of trainers and agencies to adopt truly familycentered approaches, in spite of the substantial amount of evidence of its desirability and effectiveness. She also called for learning communities that can reform current efforts at personnel preparation âin which the norm is the use of ineffective training models (episodic, short-term workshops)â (p. 111). The learning communities that Bruder envisioned are ongoing, make use of technology such as Web sites and e-mail, and are founded on mentorship and distance education models. What is at stake when personnel are inadequately prepared is poignantly demonstrated by her use of three case studies of young children (one of whom is her own nephew) served dismally by intervention staff. About him, she stated, âI am as responsible as any for the shortcomings in our field that are currently impacting his lifeâ (p. 108).
Bruderâs (2000) injection of a personal confession into the academicdiscourse is an appropriate noteon which to end this section.Itistothecreditofleadingearlyinterventiontheorists that in being invited to reflect on the state of the art of early intervention, they do not rest on their laurels. They stand united in recognizing that in spite of the very significant achievements of this fledging field, a great deal of work remains to be done to serve the growing numbers of young children in need of specialized and compensatory early childhood education.
Child Care and Early Childhood Education
The ecology of child care.
Beginning in the mid to late 1970s, a number of major changes in North America and other parts of the developed world began to have an impact on the provision of child care services and the nature of research that was conducted on child care. One major change was the sharp increase in the number and percentage of mothers of young children who worked in the paid labor force outside the home. For much of the century until this point, day nurseries were largely provided to low-income families either as part of governmental child welfare programs or by private philanthropists or religious organizations. Child care was now changing from its previous status as a welfare service for poor parents (who, it should be noted, were assumed to provide poor parenting to their children) to its emerging status as a family-support program for working parents in all income levels. Further, the programs were seen less as providing custodial baby-sitting and more as a setting in which young childrenâs early development could be stimulated and facilitated through appropriate kinds of learning activities and materials.
As pointed out in Belsky and Steinbergâs (1979) seminal literature review, another major change was the growing interest by researchers in what they referred to as modal child care programs as opposed to model programs. The model programs to which they referred tended to be situated in or affiliated with university settings. These programs tended to be well funded and staffed by well-educated professionals and to serve the children of university faculty, staff, and students. Questions began to be raised around the generalizability of findings drawn from these model programs to the more frequently usedâor modalâcommunity-based programs that tended to suffer from low and unstable funding and lower levels of staff training and that were not restricted to university-affiliated families.
Another shift was that researchers were increasingly critical of earlier studies that had reported that enrollment in child care programs could have a negative impact on motherinfant attachment patterns. Based largely on Ainsworthâs strange situation paradigm, these studies tended to focus on one-time observations of mother-infant interaction patterns in laboratory settings and typically included very little contextual data on the participants in these studies. Cross-cultural comparisons began to identify different patterns of motherchild attachment even within Western, developed countries (Lamb, 2001), calling into question the notion of a universal construct of attachment. Researchers began to examine attachment patterns within a broader range of child, maternal, family, and child care variables. The largest study of this kind was conducted by the National Institute of Child Health and Development (NICHD), a multisite study of 576 infant-mother dyads. While different patterns of attachment were found in this sample, none of the differences were attributable to the different kinds of child care arrangements in which the children participated (NICHD, 1994, 1996, 1998). In other words, there were no differences in the attachment patterns of children who were cared for by a parent in their own home or by a nonparent in a group care setting outside the home.
This shift in the attachment-related research reflected a significant shift in the nature of the research questions being posed by child care researchers. Much of the previous research on child care tended to focus on such main-effects questions such as, âIs child care good or bad for children?â This research tended to examine specific domains of child development with the childâs enrollment in a child care program as the only major independent variable. The emerging consensus was that it was not simply a childâs attendance in child care that impacted on that childâs development, but also the quality of the child care program in which the child was enrolled. Questions regarding the definition, measurement, and impact of quality child care have framed and guided much of the recent and current research. These programs of research have been guided very much by Bronfenbrennerâs (1979; Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998) work on the ecology of childhood. Bronfenbrenner argued that a childâs development is influenced by a number of concentric systems within which the child lives and grows. The microsystem includes the immediate setting in which the child is found and the nature of the experiences and activities in those settings. The mesosystem is the network and relationships among the various microsystems in which the child participates (i.e., home, child care, swim lessons, etc.). The exosystem includes these two primary systems, but its impact on the child is mediated through other individuals and institutions. Exosystem variables include legislative and policy factors that determine licensing requirements, staff education requirements, funding mechanisms, and so on. All of these systems are embedded within what Bronfenbrenner called the macrosystem, which is the virtual space in which societies and communities articulate their beliefs, attitudes, and values toward public policy regarding children and families.
Structure and Process Variables in Child Care Quality
In short, then, instead of the main-effects question (âIs child care good or bad?â), researchers began to explore the ways in which different factors from different systemic levels interacted and to determine how that interaction of factors contributed to child care quality. A number of small-scale, local studies conducted in different parts of North America in the same time frame reported a consistent and complementary set of findings regarding both the structural and the process variables that impacted on child care quality. Structural variables are those standardized, quantifiable, and regulatable variables such as group size, the adult-child ratio, licensing auspice (nonprofit or commercial), and levels of staff education. These variables were found to be significant predictors of child care quality in different sociocultural contexts using similar instruments: Los Angeles (Howes, 1987); Victoria, British Columbia (Goelman & Pence, 1987); Bermuda (Phillips, McCartney, & Scarr, 1987); and Chicago (Clarke-Stewart, 1987).
These structural factors frame the child care experience for the children and the staff and provide actors a basic foundation on which process quality could then be constructed. The term process refers to the kinds of caregiving, facilitating, educating, and playing that go on within the structural framework.
Measures of process quality extended far beyond the strange situation laboratory procedures and examined the nature of adult-child and child-child interactions in the child care setting. Goelman and Pence (1987) observed the frequency with which children engaged in activities that promote positive developmental outcomes (i.e., emergent literacy, dramatic play, fine motor play) and activities that do not (i.e., excessive television watching). Adult-child interactions were observed by Howes (1987), Phillips et al. (1987), Clarke-Stewart (1987), and others who focused on such adult characteristics as sensitivity, receptivity, detachment, and punitiveness (see Table 13.3).

Thus, in contrast to the earlier generations of child care research, which treated child care as a uniform and universal treatment variable, researchers became much more keenly aware of the tremendous diversity that exists in the daily lives of adults and children in child care settings and of how the dynamics of those settings impact both the children and the adults. Perhaps the most consistent conclusion drawn from studies of quality in child is that the concept of quality is dynamic and does not rest on any one measure, scale, or quantifiable variable.
Toward a Predictive Model of Child Care Quality
The drawback to these smaller scale local studies was that while they represented careful analyses of child care in different communities, the small numbers of children and child care programs included in these studies put serious limitations on their interpretation and generalizability. In the 1990s, therefore, a new trend in child care research was to mount research studies that included larger numbers of children from larger numbers of communities in very distinct policy and jurisdictional contexts. For example, the U.S. National Staffing Study collected data from 643 child care rooms in urban and suburban communities in Arizona, Georgia, Massachusetts, Michigan, and Washington (Howes, Phillips, & Whitebook, 1992). The U.S. Cost Quality and Outcomes Study examined quality in 604 rooms in California, Colorado, Connecticut, and North Carolina (Hellburn, 1995). The CanadianYou Bet I Care! Project examined quality in 308 rooms in nonprofit and commercial infant-toddler and preschool centers in the provinces of New Brunswick, Quebec, Ontario, Saskatchewan, Alberta, and British Columbia and in the Yukon Territory (Goelman, Doherty, Lero, LaGrange, & Tougas, 2000).Although the different studies used somewhat different sampling and instrumentation techniques, there is a strong and consistent pattern across all of these findings that both confirms and extends the findings from the earlier, smaller scale studies.
The quality of child care centers was found to be strongly linked to a combination of variables at the center, classroom, and teacher levels. Higher quality programs were found in centers that were operated as nonprofit organizations (as opposed to a commercial centers), with well trained staff both in terms of their overall levels of education and their levels of ECE-specific training. Group size and ratio provided the conditions for higher quality care, but the quality of care itself was found in distinct patterns of adult-child interaction. These patterns were characterized by heightened levels of sensitivity, responsiveness, and contingency on the part of child care staff and lower levels of punitive or detached patterns of interaction. Although it was important that the child care setting be well supplied and well stocked with developmentally appropriate materials, it was the training and education of the staff that determined whether these materials were used in the most appropriate manner. Michael Lamb (1998) summed up this body of research in this way:
Quality day care from infancy clearly has positive effects on childrenâs intellectual, verbal, and cognitive development, especially when children would otherwise experience impoverished and relatively unstimulating home environments. Care of unknown quality may have deleterious effects. (p. 104)
The identification of specific child care predictors can provide guidance and assistance to legislators, policy makers, and educators who deal with child care programs and the preparation of child care professionals. A closer look at the Canadian study (Goelman et al., 2000) suggests that whereas all of these are critical factors in child care quality, the responsibility for achieving these different quality criteria would fall to different groups of stakeholders. The quality criteria appear to fall into four distinct groups. The first group would be factors that are regulatable by local authorities: staff education levels, group size, and the adult-child ratio. Elsewhere we have argued at length for the primacy of training of staff both in terms of their overall education levels and their ECE-specific education levels (Goelman et al., 2000). Because the data from all of the studies just cited report that higher quality tends to be found in nonprofit centers than in commercial centers, there appears to be an implicit endorsement for regulatory statutes that encourage the creation of child care programs in the nonprofit rather than in the commercial sector. The establishment, implementation, and monitoring of these regulatable variables would help to provide the structural framework for quality.
A second set of variables consists of those that are related to the financial operation of the child care center. The critical financial factors were found to be staff wages, parent fees, and whether the center receives free or subsidized rent. All of these factors point to the financial vulnerability within which child care centers operate and the positive impact that is created when staff are well-compensated for their time. Subsidized or free rent helps to create additional funds that can be directed into salaries, in turn leading to lower levels of turnover. There appears also to be a set of administrative factors that can contribute significantly to child care quality. For example, the presence of student teachers from early childhood training programs has a number of positive effects on the life of the center. It assists with the adult-child ratios and brings highly motivated individuals into the center. The presence of studentteachers also helps to create a culture of inquiry and discourse among the student teachers, their supervising teachers, and their supervisors from the ECE training programs.
Finally, and as reported elsewhere, the Canadian study also found that specific attitudinal factors among the staff contributed to child care quality. Attitudes are difficult but important factors that cannot be regulated, factored into financial spreadsheets, or implemented as part of a novel administrative framework. Yet it appears that all of the three preceding categories of variables can contribute to the positive attitudes and levels of staff satisfaction that are so critical to the creation of a positive child care environment. A much-cited (but unsourced) quotation attributed to Albert Einstein claims that, âNot everything that can be counted counts. And not everything that counts can be counted.â Positive attitude may or may not be able to be assessed accurately, with validity and reliability, but the data suggest that when it can be identified, it provides a vital piece in the puzzle of quality child care.
What then, precisely, does this child care puzzle look like? Most studies of child care have relied on traditional analyses of variance, covariance, or multiple regression to bring statistical rigor to their arguments for including different and discrete pieces of the child care puzzle. Lamb (2001) and others have pointed out that in many of the child care studies the effect sizes tend to be very modest and the amount of variance accounted for is not overly impressive. Another challenge to data analyses is the determination of precisely how the variables interact. It is not clear, for example, whether the cumulative effect of these various predictors is additive, multiplicative, or exponential. For these reasons researchers are turning increasingly to more sophisticated and more powerful hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) techniques. In addition to identifying the discrete pieces of the child care puzzle, techniques such as path analysis can suggest the directionality of the paths. The metaphor of the puzzle, then, should be replaced with the image of an engine that has different parts, working together to move the vehicle forward.
Path analyses were applied to the data generated in the Canadian study (Goelman et al., 2000), and the resulting analyses identified a set of direct and indirect predictors of child care quality in rooms for infants and toddlers (0â35 months) and in rooms for 3- to 5-year-old children. Table 13.4 shows the seven direct predictors of quality in the preschool room, four staff predictors (staff wages, staff satisfaction, staff education, number of staff in the observed room), and three center predictors (whether the center receives free or subsidized rent, whether the center uses student-teachers, and the adult-child ratio in the observed room). These paths are shown graphically in Figure 13.1. The path analysis strongly suggests, however, a more complex interaction among these and other predictor variables. For example, although the auspice of the center and the parent fees were not found to be significant direct predictors of quality, their indirect impact on quality was found to be mediated through the direct predictors (see Figure 13.2). Auspice was a significant predictor of both staff wages and centers that received free or subsidized rent, both of which were found to be significant direct predictors. Parent fees were a significant predictor of wages and staff education levels, both of which, in turn, were direct predictors of quality. Finally, we note that two of the variables (staff education levels and number of staff in the observed room) served as both direct and indirect predictors of quality.

The role of child care in early childhood education continues to grow and evolve both as part of broader social and cultural changes in which the field is embedded and in terms of the practices and policies that determine the shape and content of child care programs. The demand for quality, licensed child care programs will continue to increase with the rising numbers of families with two working parents in the labor force and of single-parent families. We can expect the demand for infant child care to grow as part of this general trend. In addition, we are already witnessing an increasing demand for child care services and professionals who can respond appropriately to children with a wide range of special needs. This demand represents a challenge to create more spacesâand more appropriate spacesâfor children with special needs, and a challenge to train more early childhood educators who have the skill set and knowledge base to work with young children who have special needs. At the policy level, schools, school boards, and training institutions will have to recognize that child care is no longer remedial service for poor children or a child-minding service for the children of working parents. Child care represents a major environmental niche for the majority of young children in industrialized societies, and it is in child care settings that childrenâs development can be facilitated and supported given the right combination of predictors of quality.
Closing Thoughts on Early Childhood Education at the Beginning of the Twenty-First Century
This research paper began with a brief reflection on how early childhood education was seen at the beginning of the twentieth century and then proceeded to discuss recent, current, and emerging areas of research and practice. The field continues both to deepen and to broaden its perspectives on the innate learning and developmental abilities of the young child and the ways in which those abilities are acknowledged and facilitated in the wide range of early childhood settings in which young children participate. The developing child represents his or her world through a variety of media, modalities, and disciplines including art, reading, writing, and music. Whereas the adult world divides the world of early childhood education into content or subject areas, it seems increasingly clear that it is that complex set of behaviors, insights, expectations, and explorations known collectively as play that is the major and overarching phenomenon that infuses, guides, and largely determines what and how children learn in their early years.
What currently captures the imagination and what drives the disciplined inquiry of ECE researchers are questions about how adults and learning environments facilitate the development of a more diverse population of children than had been the focus in earlier periods. This diversity includes, but is not limited to, children at both the highest and the lowest ends of the continuum of cognitive development, the social and linguistic needs of an increasingly multicultural and immigrant early childhood population, poor children, and children whose special needs are seen as problematic but more harder to diagnose and harder still to respond to.
Early childhood theorists, researchers, and practitioners have made significant strides by acknowledging the relevance of Bronfenbrennerâs ecological systems approach to the field of early childhood. It allows for the consideration of child actions and interactions in the microsystems where the children play, learn, and grow within the broader contexts of the legislative, regulatory, and societal values in which those immediate early childhood programs are embedded. The continuing challenge to the field is to find ways of operationalizing the ecological model in ways that inform and guide emerging areas of interest, research, and practice in early childhood education.
Bibliography:
- Adams, M. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Adelman, R. D. (1990). What will I become? Play helps with the answer. Play & Culture, 3, 193â205.
- Andersen, C. J. (1990). Temperament and the child in family day care. Unpublished masterâs thesis. Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada: University of British Columbia.
- Andersen, C. J. (1994). Parent support groups. In W. B. Carey & S. C. McDevitt (Eds.), Prevention and early intervention: Individual differences as risk factors for the mental health of children (pp. 267â275). New York: Brunner/Mazel.
- Andersen, C. J. (1999). A review of formal educational opportunities in British Columbia for infant development and supported child care consultants. Victoria, British Columbia, Canada: Ministry of Advanced Education, Training and Technology.
- Andersen, C. J., & McDevitt, S. C. (2000). The temperament guides: Resources for early intervention professionals. Scottsdale, AZ: Behavioral Developmental Initiatives.
- Anderson, J. (1995). Listening to parentsâvoices: Cross cultural perceptions of learning to read and to write. Reading Horizons, 35 (5), 394â413.
- Anderson, J., & Matthews, R. (1999). Emergent storybook reading revisited. Journal of Research in Reading, 22, 293â298.
- Anderson-Goetz, D., & Worobey, J. (1984). The young childâs temperament: Implications for child care. Childhood Education, 61 (2), 134â140.
- Andress, B. (1986). Toward an integrated developmental theory for early childhood music education. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 86, 10â17.
- Andress, B. (Ed.). (1989). Promising practices: Prekindergarten music education. Reston, VA: Music Educators National Conference.
- Andress, B. (1998). Music for young children. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace.
- Arnberg, L. (1987). Raising children bilingually: The preschool years. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
- Arnett, J. (1989). Caregivers in day care centers: Does training matter? Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 10, 541â 552.
- Au, K. H. (1997). A sociocultural model of reading instruction: The Kamehameha Elementary Education Program. In S. A. Stahl & D. A. Hayes (Eds.), Instructional models in reading (pp. 181â 202). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Au, K. H., & Carroll, J. H. (1997). Improving literacy achievement through a constructivist approach: The KEEP demonstration classroom project. Elementary School Journal, 97 (3), 203â221.
- Bailey, D. B. (2000). The federal role in early intervention: Prospects for the future. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 20 (2), 71â78.
- Baker, K. A., & deKanter, A. A. (1983). An answer from research on bilingual education. American Education, 19 (6), 40â48.
- Bakhtin, M. (1981). The dialogic imagination. Austin: University of Texas.
- Bamberger, J. (1982). Growing up prodigies: The midlife crisis. New Directions for Child Development, 17, 61â77.
- Barron, A., & Earls, F. (1984). The relation of temperament and social factors to behavior problems in three-year-old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 25, 23â33.
- Bateson, G. (1955, December). A theory of play and fantasy. Psychiatric Research Reports, 2, 39â51.
- Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. New York: Ballantine.
- Belsky, J., & Steinberg, L. (1979). What does research tell us about day care? A follow-up report. Children Today, 8, 21â26.
- Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1986). Educational relevance of the study of expertise. Interchange, 17 (2), 10â19.
- Berk, L. (1985). Relationship of educational attainment, child oriented attitude, job satisfaction, and career commitment to caregiver behavior toward children. Child Care Quarterly, 14, 103â129.
- Bernhard, J. K., Lefebvre, M. L., Chud, G., & Lange, R. (1995). Paths to equity: Cultural, linguistic and racial diversity in Canadian early childhood education. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: York Lanes Press.
- Bilingual Research Journal. (1992). Washington, DC: National Association for Bilingual Education.
- Billman, J., & McDevitt, S. C. (1980). Convergence of parent and observer ratings of temperament with observations of peer interaction in nursery school. Child Development, 51, 395â400.
- Bissex, G. (1980). GYNS AT WRK: A child learns to read and write. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
- Bloch, M. N., & Pellegrini, A. D. (1989). The ecological context of childrenâs play. New York: Academic Press.
- Bloom, B. S. (1985). Developing talent in young people. New York: Ballantine.
- Boswell, J. (Ed.). (1986). Tbe young child and music: Contemporary principles in child development and music Reston, VA: Music Educators National Conference.
- Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorising sounds and learning to read: A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419â521.
- Brand, M. (1982). Relationship between musical environment and musical aptitude among sixth-grade children. (PMEA) Bulletin of Research in Music Education, 13, 13â19.
- Brand, M. (1985). Development and validation of the Home Musical Environment Scale for use at the early elementary level. Psychology of Music, 13, 40â48.
- Brand, M. (1986). Relationship between home music environment and selected musical attributes of second-grade children. Journal of Research in Music Education, 34, 111â120.
- Bredekamp, S. (1997). NAEYC issues revised position statement on Developmentally Appropriate Practices in early childhood programs. Young Children, 52, 34â40.
- Bremmer, J., & Moore, S. (1984). Prior visual inspection and object naming: Two factors that enhance hidden feature inclusion in young childrenâs drawings. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2, 371â376.
- Bricker, D. (2000). Inclusion: How the scene has changed. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 20, 14â19.
- Broen, P. A. (1972). The verbal environment of the language learning child. Monograph of American Speech and Hearing Association, 17.
- Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
- Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology of developmental processes. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theory (5th ed., pp. 993â1028). New York: Wiley.
- Bruder, M. B. (2000). Family-centered early intervention: Clarifying our values for the new millennium. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 20 (2), 105â116.
- Bruner, J. (1983). Play, thought, and language. Peabody Journal of Education, 60, 60â69.
- Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., Nabors, L. A., & Bryant, D. M. (1996). Quality of center child care and infant cognitive and language development. Child Development, 67, 606â620.
- Caillois, R. (1961). Man, play, and games. New York: Free Press.
- Campos, S. J. (1995). The Carpinteria preschool program: A longterm effects study. In E. Garcia & B. McLaughlin (Eds.), Meeting the challenge of linguistic and cultural diversity in early childhood education (pp. 34â48). New York: Teachers College Press.
- Carey, W. B. (1998). Letâs give temperament its due. Contemporary Pediatrics, 91â113.
- Carey, W. B. (2000). Estimates of temperament are easy; accurate measurements take a little longer. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 21 , 221â223.
- Carey, W. B., & McDevitt, S. C. (1989). Clinical and educational applications of temperament research. Amsterdam: Swets & Zweitlinger.
- Carey, W. B., & McDevitt, S. C. (1995). Coping with childrenâs temperament: A guide for professionals. New York: Basic.
- Ceci, S. J. (1996). On intelligence. A bioecological treatise on intellectual development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
- Center, Y., Wheldall, K., Freeman, L., Outhred, L., & McNaught, M. (1995). An evaluation of Reading Recovery. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 240â263.
- Chall, J. S. (1967). Learning to read: The great debate. New York: McGraw-Hill.
- Chapman, J. W., Tunmer, W. E., & Prochnow, J. E. (1998). Success in reading recovery depends on the development of phonological processing skills: A report prepared for the Ministry of Education. Auckland, New Zealand: Massey University.
- Chapman, M. L. (1995). Designing literacy learning experiences in a multi-age classroom. Language Arts, 72 (6), 416â428.
- Chess, S., & Thomas, A. (1987). Origins and evolutions of behavior disorders: From infancy to early adult life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
- Choi, S. C., Kim, U., & Choi, S. H. (1993). Indigenous analysis of collective representations: A Korean perspective. In U. Kim & J. W. Berry (Eds.), Indigenous psychology (pp. 193â210). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
- Chomsky, C. (1977). Approaching reading through invented spelling. In L. Resnick & P. Weaver (Eds.), Theory and practice of early reading (pp. 43â65). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Christie, J. F. (1991). Play and early literacy development: Summary and discussion. In J. F. Christie (Ed.), Play and early literacy development (pp. 233â246). Albany: State University of New York Press.
- Clark, M. (1976). Young fluent readers: What can they teach us? London: Heinemann.
- Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1987). Predicting child development from child care forms and features: The Chicago study. In D. Phillips (Ed.), Quality in child care: What does research tell us (pp. 21â42). Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children.
- Clarke-Stewart, K. A., Fitzpatrick, M. J., Allhusen, V. D., & Goldberg, W. A. (2000). Measuring difficult temperament the easy way. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 21, 207â220.
- Clay, M. (1966). Emergent reading behavior. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Auckland, New Zealand.
- Clay, M. (1979). Reading: The patterning of complex behavior (2nd ed.). Auckland, New Zealand: Heinemann Educational.
- Collins, J. (1988). Language and class in minority education. Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 19, 299â326.
- Cross, T. G. (1977). Mothersâ speech adjustments: The contribution of selected listener variables. In C. E. Snow & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press.
- Cummins, J. (1979). Linguistic interdependence and the educational development of bilingual children. Review of Educational Research, 49 (2), 222â251.
- Cummins, J. (1984). Bilingualism and special education: Issues in assessment and pedagogy. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
- Cummins, J. (1986). Empowering minority students: A framework for intervention. Harvard Education Review, 56 (1), 18â36.
- Cummins, J. (1991). The development of bilingual proficiency from home to school: A longitudinal study of Portuguese-speaking children. Journal of Education, 173 (2), 85â98.
- Cummins, J. (1992). Heritage language teaching in Canadian schools. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 24 (3), 281â286.
- Dale, P. S., Robinson, N. M., & Crain-Thoreson, C. (1995). Linguistic precocity and the development of reading: The role of extralinguistic factors. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 173â187.
- Darras, B., & Kindler, A. M. (1996). Morphogenese et teleology des images et de lâimagerie initiale. In B. Darras, Au commencement etait lâimage: Du dessin de lâenfant a la communication de lâadulte (pp. 73â94). Paris: ESF.
- Davis, J. H. (1991). Artistry lost: U-shaped development in graphic symbolization. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Harvard Graduate School of Education, Cambridge, MA.
- Davis, J. H. (1997a). Drawingâs demise: U-shaped development in graphic symbolization. Studies in Art Education, 38 (3), 132â157.
- Davis, J. H. (1997b). The âUâ and the wheel of âCâ: Development and devaluation of graphic symbolization and the cognitive approach at the Harvard Project Zero. In A. M. Kindler (Ed.), Child development in art (pp. 45â58). Reston, VA: NAEA.
- Delgado-Gaitan, C., & Trueba, H. (1991). Crossing cultural borders: Education for immigrant families in America. London: Falmer Press.
- Denzin, N. K. (1980a). Play, games, and interaction: The contexts of childhood socialization. The Sociological Quarterly, 16, 458â 478.
- Denzin, N. K. (1980b). The paradoxes of play. In J. W. Joy (Ed.), The paradoxes of play (pp. 13â24). West Point, NY: Leisure Press.
- Dewey, J. (1916). Democracy and education. NewYork: Macmillan.
- Donaldson, M. (1978). Childrenâs minds. New York: Norton Press.
- Doxey, C., & Wright, C. (1990). An exploratory study of childrenâs music ability. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 5 (3), 425â440.
- Dudley-Marling, C., & Murphy, S. (1997). Apolitical critique of remedial reading programs: The example of Reading Recovery. The Reading Teacher, 50, 460â468.
- Duncum, P. (1986). Breaking down the U-curve of artistic development. Visual Arts Research, 12 (1), 43â54.
- Dunst, C. J. (2000). Revisiting âRethinking Early Intervention.â Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 20 (2), 95â104.
- Durkin, D. (1966). Children who read early. New York: Teachers College Press.
- Dyson, A. H. (1997). Writing superheroes: Contemporary childhood, popular culture, and classroom literacy. New York: Teachers College Press.
- Earls, F., & Jung, K. G. (1987). Temperament and home environment as causal factors in the development of childhood psychopathology. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 491â498.
- Edwards, L. C. (1990). Affective development and creative arts. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Merrill.
- Elster, C. (1994). Patterns within preschoolers emergent reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 29, 409â425.
- Erhmann, J. (Ed.). (1968). Game, play, literature. Boston: Beacon Press.
- Faulstich Orellana, M. (1994). Appropriating the voice of the superheroes: Three preschoolersâ bilingual language uses in play. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 9, 171â193.
- Feldman, D. H. (1986). Natureâs gambit: Child prodigies and the development of human potential. New York: Basic Books.
- Ferreiro, E., & Teberosky, A. (1982). Literacy before schooling. Exeter, NH: Heinemann.
- Field, T., & Greenberg, R. (1982). Temperament ratings by parents and teachers of infants, toddlers, and preschool children. Child Development, 53, 160â163.
- Fineberg, J. (1997 ). The innocent eye: Childrenâs art and the modern artist. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
- Fischer, K. W., & Canfield, R. L. (1986). The ambiguity of stage and structure of behavior: Person and environment in the development of psychological structure. In I. Levin (Ed.), Stage and structure: Reopening the debate (pp. 246â367). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
- Fischer, K. W., Knight, C. C., & Van Parys, M. (1993). Analyzing diversity in developmental pathways: Methods and concepts. In R. Case & W. Edelstein (Eds.), The new structuralism in cognitive development: Theory and research on individual pathways (pp. 33â56). Basel, Switzerland: Karger.
- Flohr, J. W. (1981). Short-term music instruction and young childrenâs developmental music aptitude. Journal of Research in Music Education, 29, 219â223.
- Franciscono, M. (1998). Paul Klee and childrenâs art. In J. Fineberg (Ed.), Discovering child art (pp. 95â121). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
- Franyo, G., & Hyson, M. (1999). Temperament training for early childhood caregivers: A study of the effectiveness of training. Child and Youth Care Forum, 28, 329â349.
- Freeman, J. (2000). Teaching for talent: Lessons from the research. In C. F. M. van Lieshout & P. G. Heymans (Eds.), Developing talent across the life span (pp. 231â248). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
- Freud, S. (1909). The complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud: Vol. 10. Analysis of a phobia in a five-year-old boy . London: Hogarth Press.
- Froebel, F. (1885). The education of man (J. Jarvis, Trans.). New York: Lovell.
- Gaitskell, C. D., Hurwitz, A., & Day, M. (1982). Children and their art (4th ed.). New York: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.
- Galda, L., & Pellegrini, A. D. (1985). Play, language, and stories. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
- Garcia, E. E. (1988). Effective schooling for language minority students . Arlington, VA: National Clearing House for Bilingual Education.
- Garcia, E. E. (1993). The education of linguistically and culturally diverse children. In B. Spodek (Ed.), Handbook of research on the education of young children (pp. 372â384). New York: Macmillan.
- Gardner, H. (1980). Artful scribbles: The significance of childrenâs drawings. New York: Basic Books.
- Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind. The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books.
- Gardner, H., & Winner, E. (1982). First intimations of artistry. In S. Strauss (Ed.), U-shaped behavioral growth. New York: Academic Press.
- Garvey, C. (1977). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
- C. (1972). The interpretation of cultures. New York: Basic Books.
- Goelman, H., Doherty, G., Lero, D., LaGrange, & Tougas, J. (2000). Caring and learning in child care centres across Canada. Guelph, Ontario, Canada: University of Guelph Centre for Families, Work and Well-Being.
- Goelman, H., & Jacobs, E. V. (1994). Childrenâs play in childcare settings. Albany: State University of New York Press.
- Goelman, H., & Pence, A. R. (1987). Effects of child care, family and individual characteristics on childrenâs language development: The Victoria Day Care Research Project. In D. A. Phillips (Ed.), What does research tell us? (pp. 89â104). Washington, DC: National Association of the Education of Young Children.
- Goelman, H., Shapiro, E. & Pence, A. R. (1992). Family environment and family day care. Family Relations, 4 (19), 251â270.
- Goetze, M., Cooper, N., & Brown, C. J. (1990). Recent research on singing in the general music classroom. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 104, 16â37.
- Goldenberg, C., Reese, L., & Gallimore, R. (1992). Effects of school literacy materials on Latino childrenâs home experiences and early reading achievement. American Journal of Education, 100 (4), 497â536.
- Golomb, C. (1992). The childâs creation of a pictorial world. Berkeley: University of California Press.
- Golomb, C. (1994). Drawing as representation: The childâs acquisition of a meaningful graphic language. Visual Arts Research, 20 (2), 14â28.
- Goodnow, J. J. (1988). Parentsâideas, actions, and feelings: Models and methods from developmental and social psychology. Child Development, 59, 286â320.
- Gordon, E. E. (1978). Primary measures of music audiation. Chicago: G.I.A.
- Gordon, E. E. (1979). Developmental aptitude as measured by the Primary Measures of Music Audiation. Psychology of Music, 7, 42â49.
- Gordon, E. E. (1986a). Intermediate measures of music audiation. Chicago: G.I.A.
- Gordon, E. E. (1986b). The nature, description, measurement, and evaluation of music aptitudes. Chicago: G.I.A.
- Gordon, E. E. (1989). Advanced measures of music audiation. Chicago: G.I.A.
- Gordon, E. E. (1990). A music learning theory for newborn and young children. Chicago: G.I.A.
- Gordon, E. E. (1993). Learning sequences in music: Skill, content, and patterns. Chicago: G.I.A.
- Gottfried, A. W., Gottfried, A. E., Bathurst, K., & Guerin, D. W. (1994). Gifted IQ: Early developmental aspects. The Fullerton longitudinal study. New York: Plenum Press.
- Gouzouasis, P. J. (1987). The comparative effects of three types of accompaniment on the singing achievement and developmental tonal aptitude of young children. Unpublished masterâs thesis, Temple University, Philadelphia.
- Gouzouasis, P. J. (1991). A progressive developmental approach to the music education of preschool children. Canadian Music Educator, 32 (3), 45â53.
- Gouzouasis, P. J. (1992). An organismic model of music learning for young children. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 11 (1), 13â18.
- Gouzouasis, P. J. (1993). Music aptitude: Acomparison of the music abilities of kindergarten children of various ethnic backgrounds. The Quarterly Journal of Music Teaching and Learning, 4 (2), 70â76.
- Gouzouasis, P. J. (1994). A developmental model of music literacy. Research Forum, 12 (spring), 21â24.
- Gouzouasis, P. J. (2000). Understanding music media: Digital (re)genesis or cultural meltdown in the 21st century. In B. Hanley & B. A. Roberts (Eds.), Looking forward: Challenges to Canadian music education. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Hushin House.
- Gouzouasis, P. J., & Taggart, C. (1995). The music learning and language learning metaphor: An organismic perspective. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 13 (2), 9â13.
- Graham, P., Rutter, M., & George, S. (1973). Temperamental characteristics as predictors of behavior disorders in children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 43, 328â339.
- Grant, R. (1995). Meeting the needs of young second language learners. In E. Garcia & B. McLaughlin (Eds.), Meeting the challenge of linguistic and cultural diversity in early childhood education (pp. 1â17). New York: Teachers College Press.
- Graves, D. (1983). Writing: Teachers and children at work. Exeter, NH: Heinemann.
- Greenspan, S. (1997). Growth of the mind. New York: Addison Wesley.
- Guilmartin, K., & Levinowitz, L. M. (1989â2000). Music Together Curriculum. Princeton, NJ: Birch Tree Group.
- Hakuta, K. (1986). Mirror of language: The debate of bilingualism. New York: Basic Books.
- Harden, B., Winslow, M., Kendziora, K., Shahinfar, A., Rubin, K., Fox, N., Crowley, M., and Zahn-Waxler, C. (2000). Externalizing problems in Head Start children: An ecological exploration. Early Education and Development, 11, 357â385.
- Hart, B. (2000). A natural history of early language experience. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 20 (1), 28â35.
- Hartley, R. E., & Goldenson, R. M. (1963). The complete book of childrenâs play (Rev. ed.). New York: Crowell.
- Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life, and work in communities and classroom. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
- Heath,S.B.,&Thomas,C.(1984).Theachievementofpreschoolliteracy for mother and child. In H. Goelman,A. Oberg, & F. Smith (Eds.), Awakening to literacy (pp. 51â72). Portsmouth, NH: Heinman.
- Helburn,S.W.(Ed).(1995). Cost,qualityandchildoutcomesinchild care centers. Denver: University of Colorado at Denver, Department of Economics, Center for Research and Social Policy.
- Hofmann, W. (1998). The art of unlearning. In J. Fineberg (Ed.), Discovering child art (pp. 3â14). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
- Holdaway, D. (1979). The foundations of literacy. Sydney, Australia: Ashton Scholastic.
- Hollingworth, L. S. (1942). Children above 180 IQ. Yonkers, NY: World Book.
- Holloway, S. D., & Reichhart-Erickson, M. (1988). The relationship of day care quality to childrenâs free play behavior and social problem-solving skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 3, 39â53.
- Holohan, J. M. (1984, June). The development of music syntax: Some observations of music babble of young children. Paper delivered at the Music in Early Childhood Conference, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT.
- Howes, C. (1983). Caregiver behavior in center and family day care. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 4, 99â107.
- Howes, C. (1987) Quality indicators in infant and toddler child care: The Los Angeles Study. In D. A. Phillips (Ed.), Quality in child care: What does research tell us? (pp. 81â88). Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children.
- Howes, C. (1997). Childrenâs experiences in center-based child care as a function of teacher background and adult:child ratio. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 43, 404â425.
- Howes, C., Phillips, D. A., & Whitebook, M. (1992). Thresholds of quality: Implications for the social development of children in center-based child care. Child Development, 63, 449â460.
- Howes, C., & Smith, E. W. (1995). Relations among child care quality, teacher behavior, childrenâs play activities, emotional security, and cognitive activity in child care. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 10, 381â404.
- Hunt, J. M. (1961). Intelligence and experience. New York: Ronald Press.
- Jacob, E. J. (1984). Learning literacy through play: Puerto Rican kindergarten children. In H. Goelman, A. Oberg, & F. Smith (Eds.), Awakening to literacy. Exeter, NH: Heinemann.
- Jakobson, R. (1968). Child language, aphasia, and phonological universals (A. R. Keiler, Trans.). The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton.
- Jersild, A. T., & Bienstock, S. F. (1931). The influence of training on the vocal ability of three-year-old children. Child Development, 2, 272â291.
- Jersild, A. T., & Bienstock, S. F. (1934). Astudy of the development of childrenâs ability to sing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 25, 481â503.
- Jessup, L. L. (1984). The comparative effects of indirect and direct musicteachinguponthedevelopmentalmusicaptitudeandmusic achievement of early primary children (Doctoral dissertation, Temple University, 1984). Dissertation Abstracts International, 45,
- Kagan, S. L., & Garcia, E. E. (1991). Educating culturally and linguistically diverse preschoolers: Moving the agenda. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 6, 427â443.
- Kandinsky,V.(1912).ĂberdieFormfrage. DerBlaueReiter,168,
- Keating, D. P. (1991). Curriculum options for the developmentally advanced: A developmental alternative to gifted education. Exceptionality Education Canada, 1, 53â84.
- Kelly-Byrne, K. (1989). A childâs play life: An ethnographic study. New York: Teachers College Press.
- Keogh, B. K., & Burstein, N. D. (1988). Relationship of temperament to preschoolersâ interactions with peers and teachers. Exceptional Children 54 (5), 456â461.
- Kim, U., & Choi, S. H. (1994). Individualism, collectivism, and child development: A Korean perspective. In P. M. Greenfield & R. R. Cocking (Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child development (pp. 227â257). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Kindler, A. M. (1996). Myths, habits, research and policy: The four pillars of early childhood art education. Arts Education Policy Review, 97 (4), 24â30.
- Kindler, A. M. (1997). Directions in primary and intermediate art education. BCATA Journal for Art Teacher, 37 (1), 30â37.
- Kindler, A. M. (1998). Artistic development and art education. Translations, 7( 2), 1â6.
- Kindler, A. M. (1999). âFrom endpoints to repertoiresâ: Achallenge to art education. Studies in Art Education, 40 (4), 330â349.
- Kindler, A. M. (2001). From the u-curve to dragons: Culture and understanding of artistic development. Visual Arts Research, 26 (2), 15â29.
- Kindler, A. M., & Darras, B. (1997a). Development of pictorial representation: Ateleology-based model. Journal of Art and Design Education, 16 (3), 217â222.
- Kindler A. M., & Darras, B. (1997b). Map of artistic development. In A. M. Kindler (Ed.), Child development in art (pp. 17â24). Reston, VA: NAEA.
- Kindler, A. M., & Darras, B. (1998). Culture and development of pictorial repertoires. Studies in Art Education, 39 (2), 147â166.
- Klee, P. (1957). Tagebucher von Paul Klee 1898â1918. Cologne: Verlag M. DuMont Schauberg.
- Koh, J. (2000). Korean immigrant parentsâ attitudes and practices regarding Korean and English development in preschool-aged children: A comparison of bilingual and English-speaking programs. Unpublished masterâs thesis, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.
- Kontos, S., Howes, C., & Galinsky, E. (1996). Does training make a difference to quality in family child care? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 11, 427â445.
- Korzenik, D. (1995). The changing concept of artistic giftedness. In C. Golomb (Ed.), The development of artistically gifted children (pp. 1â30). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- LaGrange, A., Clark, D., & Munroe, E. (1995). Culturally sensitive child care: The Alberta study. Edmonton, Alberta, Canada: Alberta Association for Young Children.
- Lamb, M. E. (2001, March 5). Developmental theory and public policy: A cross-national perspective. Lecture given as part of the Green College Distinguished Lecture Series on, âMultiple lenses, multiple images: Perspectives on the child across time, space and disciplines.â Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.
- Lamb, M. E. (1998). Nonparental child care: Context, quality, correlates, and consequences. In W. Damon, I. E. Sigel, & K. A. Renninger (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Child psychology in practice (pp. 73â133). New York: Wiley.
- Lambert, W. E., & Taylor, D. M. (1983). Language in the education of ethnic minority immigrants. In R. J. Samuda & S. L. Woods (Eds.), Perspectives in immigrant and minority education (pp. 267â280). Washington, DC: University Press of America.
- Lee, R., & Lehmann, F. (1986). Korean immigrants in British Columbia. In K. V. Ujimoto & J. Naidoo (Eds.), Asian Canadian Symposium: Vol. 7. Asian Canadian: Contemporary issues (pp. 49â68). Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada: University of Manitoba Press.
- Levinowitz, L. M. (1985). The comparative effects of two types of song instruction on the development of a sense of tonality in fouryear-old children. Unpublished masterâs thesis, Temple University, Philadelphia.
- Levinowitz, L. M. (1987). An experimental study of the comparative effects of singing songs with words and without words on children in kindergarten and first grade (Doctoral dissertation, Temple University, 1987). Dissertation Abstracts International, 48,
- Littleton, D. (1998). Music learning and childâs play. General Music Today, 12 (1), 8â15.
- Louis, B., & Lewis, M. (1992). Parental beliefs about giftedness in young children and their relation to actual ability level. Gifted Child Quarterly, 36, 27â31.
- Lowenfeld, V., & Brittain, W. L. (1964). Creative and mental growth (4th ed.). New York: Macmillan.
- Luke, A., Baty, A., & Stehbens, C. (1989). Natural conditions for language learning: A critique. English in Australia, 89, 36â49.
- Luke, A., & Luke, C. (2001). Adolescence lost/childhood regained: On early intervention and the emergence of the techno-subject. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 1, 91â120.
- Luquet, G. H. (1977). Le dessin enfantin. Paris: Delachaux et Niestle.
- Lyon, J. (1996). Becoming bilingual: Language acquisition in a bilingual community. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
- Lyon, M., & Canning, P. (1995). The Atlantic Day Care Study. StJohnâs,Newfoundland:MemorialUniversityofNewfoundland.
- Mace, J. (1998). Playing with time: Mothers and the meaning of literacy. London: UCL Press.
- Malicky, G. V., & Norman, C. A. (1999). Phonological awareness and reading: An alternative interpretation of the literature from a clinical perspective. Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 45 (1), 18â34.
- Mason, J. (1992). Reading stories to preliterate children: A proposed connection to reading. In P. Gough, L. Ehri, & R. Truman (Eds.), Reading Acquisition (pp. 215â239). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Mason, J., & Kerr, B. (1992). Literacy transfer from parents to children in the preschool years. In T. Sticht, M. Beeler, & B. McDonald (Eds.), The intergenerational transfer of cognitive skills: Vol. 2. Theory and research in cognitive science. Norwood, NJ: ABLEX.
- Maziade, M., Caron, C., Cote, R., Boutin, P., & Thivierge, J. (1990). Extreme temperament and diagnosis: A study in a psychiatric sample of consecutive children. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47, 477â484.
- Maziade, M., Cote, R., Bernier, H., Boutin, P., & Thivierge, J. (1989). Significance of extreme temperament in infancy for clinical status in pre-school years. British Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 544â551.
- McCollum, J. A. (2000). Taking the past along: Reflecting on our identity as a discipline. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 20 (2), 79â86.
- McKim, M. K., Cramer, K. M., Stuart, B., & OâConnor, D. L. (1999). Infant care decisions and attachment security: The Canadian âTransition to Child Careâ Study. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 31 (2), 92â106.
- Meichenbaum, D., & Biemiller, A. (1998). Nurturing independent learners. Helping students take charge of their learning. Cambridge, MA: Brookline Books.
- Michaels, S. (1981). âSharing timeâ: Childrenâs narrative styles and differential access to literacy. Language in Society, 10, 423â442. Michelet, A. (1986). Teachers and play. Prospects, 16, 113â122.
- Miller, S. M., Fernie, D., & Kantor, K. (1992). Distinctive literacies in different preschool play contexts. Play and Culture, 5, 107â 119.
- Montessori, M. (1965). Montessoriâs own handbook. New York: Schoken. (Original work published 1914)
- Moorhead, G., & Pond, D. (1978). Music of young children. Santa Barbara, CA: Pillsbury Foundation for the Advancement of Music Education.
- Morelock, M. J. (1996). On the nature of giftedness and talent: Imposing order on chaos. Roeper Review, 19, 4â12.
- Morgan, M. (1988). Art 4-11: Art in early years of schooling. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell.
- Music Educators National Conference. (1986). The school music program. Descriptions and standards. Reston, VA: Author.
- Music Educators National Conference. (1991). Issues in music education: An advisory from the Music Educators National Conference. Reston, VA. Author.
- Mustafa, M. (1997). Beyond traditional phonics . Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
- National Institute of Child Health and Development, Early Child Care Network. (1994). Child care and child development: The NICHD study of early child care. In S. L. Friedman & H. C. Haywood (Eds.), Developmental follow-up: Concepts, domains, and methods (pp. 378â396). New York: Academic Press.
- NationalInstituteofChildHealthandDevelopment,EarlyChildCare Research Network. (1996). Characteristics of infant child care: Factors contributing to positive caregiving. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 11, 269â306.
- National Institute of Child Health and Development, Early Child Care Research Network. (1998). Early child care and selfcontrol, compliance, and problem behaviors at twenty-four and thirty-six months. Child Development, 69 (4), 1145â1170.
- National Society for the Study of Education. (1929). Preschool and parental education. Bloomington, IL: Public School.
- National Society for the Study of Education. (1939). Intelligence: Its nature and nurture. Bloomington, IL.: Public School Company.
- Neuman, S. B., & Roskos, K. (1992). Literacy objects as cultural tools: Effects on childrenâs literacy behaviours in play. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 202â225.
- Neuman, S. B., & Roskos, K. (1993). Access to print for children of poverty: Differential effects of adult mediation and literacyenriched play settings on environmental and functional print tasks. American Educational Research Journal, 30, 95â122.
- Newport, E. L., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. R. (1977). Mother, Iâd rather do it myself: Some effects and noneffects of maternal speech style. In C. E. Snow & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 109â150). New York: Cambridge University Press.
- Odom, S. (2000). Preschool inclusion: What we know and where we go from here. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 20 (1), 20â29.
- Palison, H. (1986). Preschool temperament and performance on achievement tests. Developmental Psychology, 22, 766â770.
- Pariser, D., & van den Berg, A. (1997). The mind of the beholder: Some provisional doubts about the u-curve aesthetic development thesis. Studies in Art Education, 38 (3), 158â178.
- Parsons, M. J. (1998). Intergrated curriculum and our paradigm of cognition in the arts. Studies in Art Education, 39 (2), 103â116.
- Paul, B., & Jarvis, C. (1992). The effects of native language use in New York city pre-kindergarten classes. Paper presented at the 1992 Annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco.
- Pellegrini, A. D. (1991). A critique of the concept of at risk as applied to emergent literacy. Language Arts, 68, 380â385.
- Pellegrini, A. D. (1984). The effect of dramatic play on childrenâs generation of cohesive text. Discourse Processes, 7, 57â 67.
- Pellegrini, A. D., & Galda, L. (1998). The development of schoolbased literacies: A social ecological perspective. New York: Routledge.
- Pellegrini, A. D., Galda, L., Dresden, J., & Cox, S. (1991). A longitudinal study of the predictive relations among symbolic play, linguistic verbs, and early literacy. Research in the Teaching of English, 25, 219â235.
- Peller, L. E. (1952). Models of childrenâs play. Mental Hygiene, 36, 66â83.
- Pestalozzi, H. (1827). Letters on early education. London: Charles Gilpin.
- Pettengill, S. M., & Rohner, R. P. (1985). Korean-American adolescentsâ perceptions of parental control, parental acceptancerejection and parent-adolescent conflict. In I. R. Lanunes & Y. H. Poortinga (Eds.), From different perspectives: Studies of behavior across cultures (pp. 241â249). Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.
- Phillips, D. A., & Howes, C. (1987). Indicators of quality child care: Review of research. In D. A. Phillips (Ed.), Quality in child care: What does research tell us? Washington, DC: National Association of the Education of Young Children.
- Phillips, D. A., McCartney, K., & Scarr, S. (1987). Child-care quality and childrenâs social development. Developmental Psychology, 23, 537â544.
- Phillips, J. R. (1970). Formal characteristics of speech which mothers address to their young children. (Doctoral dissertation, Johns Hopkins University, 1970). Dissertation Abstracts International, 31,
- Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.
- Pond, D. (1981). Acomposerâs study of young childrenâs innate musicality. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 68, 1â12.
- Porath, M. (1986). Gifted childrenâs responses to research participation. Unpublished raw data.
- Porath, M. (1992). Stage and structure in the development of children with various types of âgiftedness.â In R. Case (Ed.), The mindâs staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of childrenâs thought and knowledge (pp. 303â317). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
- Porath, M. (1993). Gifted young artists: Developmental and individual differences. Roeper Review, 16, 29â33.
- Porath, M. (1996). Affective and motivational consideration in the assessment of gifted learners. Roeper Review, 19, 13â17.
- Porath, M. (1996). Narrative performance in verbally gifted children. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 19, 276â292.
- Porath, M. (1997). A developmental model of artistic giftedness in middle childhood. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 20, 201â223.
- Porath, M. (2000). Social giftedness in childhood: A developmental perspective. In R. C. Friedman & B. M. Shore (Eds.), Talents unfolding: Cognitive and developmental frameworks (pp. 195â 215). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
- Porath, M. (2001). Young girlsâ social understanding: Emergent interpersonal expertise. High Ability Studies, 12, 113â126.
- Porter, R. P. (1991). Language choice for Latino students. The Public Interest, 105, 48â60.
- Pressley, M., El-Dinary, P. B., Marks, M. B., Brown, R., & Stein, S. (1992). Good strategy instruction is motivating and interesting. In K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 333â358). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Pressley, M., Wharton-McDonald, R., Allington, R., Block, C. C., Morrow, L., Tracy, D., Baker, K., Brooks, G., Cronin, J., Nelson, E., & Woo, D. (1998). The nature of effective first-grade literacy instruction. (Research Rep. No. 11007). Albany, NY: University of New York at Albany, National Research Center on English Learning & Achievement.
- Pressley, M., & Woloshyn, V. (1995 ). Cognitive strategy instruction that really improves childrenâs academic performance (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: Brookline Books.
- Prince, C., & Lawrence, L. (1994). School readiness and language minority students: Implications of the first national education goal. Washington, DC: NCBE.
- Purcell-Gates, V. (1996). Stories, coupons, and the âTV Guideâ: Relationships between home literacy experiences and emergent literacy knowledge. Reading Research Quarterly, 31 (4), 406â428.
- Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., Levine, L. J., Wright, E. L., Dennis, W. R., & Newcomb, R. (1997). Music training causes long-term enhancement of preschool childrenâs spatial-temporal reasoning abilities. Neurological Research,19, 1â8.
- Read, C. (1975). Childrenâs categorization of speech sounds in Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.
- Reyes, M. de la Luz. (1992). Challenging venerable assumptions: Literacy instruction for linguistically different students. Harvard Educational Review, 62, 427â445.
- Robinson, N. M. (1987). The early development of precocity. Gifted Child Quarterly, 31, 161â164.
- Robinson, N. M. (1993). Identifying and nurturing gifted, very young children. In K. A. Heller, F. J. Monks, & A. H. Passow (Eds.), Research and development of giftedness and talent (pp. 507â524). Tarrytown, NY: Pergamon.
- Robinson, N. M. (2000). Giftedness in very young children: How seriously should it be taken? In R. C. Friedman & B. M. Shore (Eds.), Talents unfolding: Cognition and development (pp. 7â26). Washington, DC:American PsychologicalAssociation.
- Robinson, N. M., Abbott, R. D., Berninger, V. W., & Busse, J. (1996). The structure of abilities in math-precocious young children: Gender similarities and differences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 341â352.
- Robinson, N. M., Abbott, R. D., Berninger, V. W., Busse, J., & Mukhopadhyay, S. (1997). Developmental changes in mathematically precocious young children: Longitudinal and gender effects. Gifted Child Quarterly, 41, 145â158.
- Robinson, N. M., & Noble, K. D. (1991). Social-emotional development and adjustment of gifted children. In M. G. Wang, M. C. Reynolds, & H. J. Walberg (Eds.), Handbook of special education: Vol. 4. Research and practice (pp. 23â36). New York: Pergamon.
- Robinson, N. M., & Robinson, H. B. (1982). The optimal match: Devising the best compromise for the highly gifted student. In D. Feldman (Ed.), New directions for child development: Vol. 17. Developmental approaches to giftedness and creativity (pp. 79â94). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
- Rodriguez, J., Diaz, R., Duran, D., & Espinosa, L. (1995). The impact of bilingual preschool education on the language development of Spanish-speaking children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 10, 475â490.
- Roskos, K. (1988). Literacy at work in play. The Reading Teacher, 41, 562â566.
- Roskos, K., & Neuman, S. B. (1993). A typology of young childrenâs literacy activity in play. Journal of Play Theory and Research, 1 (1), 17â25.
- Rutter, M. (1982). Temperament: Concepts, issues and problems. In R. Porter & G. M. Collins (Eds.), Temperamental differences in infants and young children, Ciba Foundation Symposium 89 (pp. 1â16). London: Pitman.
- Rutter, M. (1994). Temperament: Changing concepts and implications. In W. B. Carey & S. C. McDevitt (Eds.), Prevention and early intervention: Individual differences as risk factors for the mentalhealthofchildren (pp.23â34).NewYork:Brunner/Mazel.
- Santa, C., & Hoien, T. (1999). An assessment of early steps: A program for early intervention of reading problems. Reading Research Quarterly, 34, 54â79.
- Saracho, O. N., & Spodek, B. (1995). Childrenâs play and early childhood education: Insights from history and theory. Journal of Education, 177 (3), 129â49.
- Scarborough, H. S., & Dobrich, W. (1994). On the efficacy of reading to preschoolers. Developmental Review, 14, 245â302.
- Scarr, S., Eisenberg, M., & Deater-Deckard, K. (1994). Measurement of quality in child care centers. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 9, 131â151.
- Schiff-Myers, N. B. (1992). Considering arrested language development and language loss in the assessment of secondl anguage Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 23, 28â33.
- Schmidt, P. R. (1998). Cultural conflict and struggle: Literacy learning in a kindergarten program. New York: P. Lang.
- Schrader, C. T. (1991). Symbolic play: A source of meaningful engagements with writing and reading. In J. F. Christie (Ed.), Play and early literacy development (pp. 189â213). Albany: State University of New York Press.
- Schwartzman, H. B. (1978). Transformations: The anthropology of childrenâs play. New York: Plenum.
- Schwartzman, H. B. (1991). Imagining play. Play and Culture, 4 (3), 214â222.
- Shaw, G. L. (1999). Keeping Mozart in mind. New York: Academic Press.
- Shore, R. (1997). Rethinking the brain: New insights into early development. New York: Families and Work Institute.
- Simons, G. (1986). Early childhood music development:Asurvey of selected research. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 86, 36â52.
- Siren, U. (1991). Minority language transmission in early childhood: Parental intention and language use. Stockholm, Sweden: Stockholm University Press.
- Skutnabb-Kangas, T. (1981). Bilingualism or not: The education of minorities. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
- Smith, B. J. (2000). The federal role in early childhood special education policy in the next century: The responsibility of the individual. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 20 (1), 7â11.
- Smitsman, A. W. (2000). Slumbering talents: Where do they reside? In C. F. M. van Lieshout & P. G. Heymans (Eds.), Developing talent across the life span (pp. 17â40). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
- Snow, C. E. (1977). Mothersâ speech research: From input to interaction. In C. E. Snow & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press.
- Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
- Soderman, A. K. (1985, July). Dealing with difficult young children: Strategies for teachers and parents. Young Children, 41, 15â20.
- Soto, L. D. (1993). Native language for school success. Bilingual Research Journal, 17, 83â97.
- Soto, L. D. (1997). Language, culture, and power: Bilingual families and the struggle for quality education. New York: SUNY Press.
- Southern, W. T., & Jones, E. D. (1991). The academic acceleration of gifted children. New York: Teachers College Press.
- Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360â406.
- Stansbury, K., & Harris, M. L. (2000). Individual differences in stress reactions during a peer entry episode: Effects of age, temperament, approach behavior, and self-perceived peer competence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 76 (1), 50â63.
- Stark, R. E. (1977). Features of infant sounds: The emergence of cooing. Journal of Child Language, 5, 379â390.
- Stark, R. E., Rose, S. N., & McClagen, A. (1975). Features of infant sounds: The first eight weeks of life. Journal of Child Language, 2, 205â222.
- Strain, P. S., & Hoyson, M. (2000). The need for longitudinal, intensive social skills intervention: LEAP follow-up outcomes for children with autism. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 20 (2), 116â122.
- Sulzby, E. (1985). Childrenâs emergent reading of favourite storybooks: A developmental study. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 427â446.
- Sulzby, E., & Teale, W. (1991). Emergent literacy. In R. Barr, M. Kamil, P. Mosenthall, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research: Vol. 2 (pp. 727â757). New York: Longman.
- Takhvar, M. (1988). Play and theories of play: A review of the literature. Early Child Development and Care, 39, 221â244.
- Taylor, D. (1983). Family literacy. Exeter, NH: Heinemann.
- Teale, W., & Sulzby, E. (1986). Emergent literacy as a perspective for examining how young children become readers and writers. In W. Teale & E. Sulzby (Eds.), Emergent literacy: Writing and reading (pp. viiâxxv). Norwood, MA: Ablex.
- Templeton, S., & Morris, D. (1999). Questions teachers ask about spelling. Reading Research Quarterly, 34, 102â112.
- There is no Utopia: The story of returning immigrants. (1999, December 23). The Hangyurea Newsletter, Issue No. 288. Retrieved from http://www.hani.co.kr/h21/data/l991213/1pascd 01 .html.
- Vandell, D. L., &. Corasaniti, M. A. (1990). Variations in early child care: Do they predict subsequent social, emotional, and cognitive differences? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 5, 55â72.
- Villarruel, F. A., Imig, D. R., & Kostelnik, M. J. (1995). Diverse families. In E. Garcia & B. McLaughlin (Eds.), Meeting the challenge of linguistic and cultural diversity in early childhood education (pp. 103â124). New York: Teachers College Press.
- Volk, D. (1997). Questions in lessons: Activity settings in the homes and school of two Puerto Rican kindergarteners. Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 28 (1), 22â49.
- Vygotsky, L. S. (1976). Play and its role in the mental development of the child. In J. Bruner, J. Jolly, & K. Sylva (Eds.), Play: Its role in development and evolution (pp. 537â554). New York: Basic Books.
- Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in a society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
- Wang, S. (1987). Yani as I see her. In Wang Yani: Pictures by a young Chinese girl (pp. 15â23). Munich: Prestel.
- Weisner, T. S., Gallimore, R., & Jordan, C. (1993). Unpacking cultural effects of classroom learning: Hawaiian peer assistance and child-generated activity. In R. N. Roberts (Ed.), Coming home to preschool: The sociocultural context of early education (pp. 59â87). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
- Wells, G. (1985). Preschool literacy-related activities and success in school. In D. R. Olson, N. Torrence, & A. Hildyard (Eds.), Literacy language and learning: The nature and consequences of reading and writing (pp. 229â255). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
- Whitebook, M., Howes, C., & Phillips, D. (1990). Who cares? Child care teachers and the quality of care in America. Final Report, National Child Care Staffing Study. Oakland, CA: Child Care Employee Project.
- Wilson, B., & Wilson, M. (1977). An iconoclastic view of the imagery sources in the drawings of young people. Art Education, 30 (1), 5â11.
- Wilson, B., & Wilson, M. (1981). Review of Artful scribbles: The significance of childrenâs drawings, by H. Gardner. Studies in Visual Communication, 7 (1), 86â89.
- Winner, E. (1996). Gifted children: Myths and realities. New York: Basic Books.
- Winton, P. J. (2000). Early childhood intervention personnel preparation: Backward mapping for future planning. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 20 (2), 87â94.
- Wolf, D. (1994). Development as growth of repertoires. In M. F. Franklin & B. Kaplan (Eds.), Development and the arts (pp. 59â78). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
- Wolf, D., & Perry, M. (1988). From endpoints to repertoires: New conclusions about drawing development. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 22 (1), 17â35.
- Wong Fillmore, L. (1991). When learning a second language means losing the first? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 6, 323â346.
- Wright, S. C., & Taylor, D. M. (1995). Identity and the language of the classroom: Investing the impact of heritage versus second language instruction on personal and collective self-esteem. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 241â252.
- Wright, S. C., Taylor, D. M., & Macarthur, J. (2000). Subtractive bilingualism and the survival of the Inuit language: Heritageversus second-language education. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92 (1), 63â84.
- Yen, C. C., & Ispa, J. M. (2000). Childrenâs temperament and behavior in Montessori and constructivist early childhood programs. Early Education and Development, 11 (2), 171â186.
- Zigler, E., & Styfco, S. J. (2000). Pioneering steps (and fumbles) in developing a federal preschool intervention. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 20 (2), 67â70.
- Zimmerman, M. P. (1986). Music development in middle childhood: A summary of selected research studies. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 86, 18â35.
ORDER HIGH QUALITY CUSTOM PAPER

We use cookies to enhance our website for you. Proceed if you agree to this policy or learn more about it.
- Essay Database >
- Essays Examples >
- Essay Topics
Essays on Kindergarten
35 samples on this topic
Writing a lot of Kindergarten papers is an essential part of present-day studying, be it in high-school, college, or university. If you can do that single-handedly, that's just awesome; yet, other students might not be that fortunate, as Kindergarten writing can be quite challenging. The collection of free sample Kindergarten papers introduced below was formed in order to help struggling students rise up to the challenge.
On the one hand, Kindergarten essays we publish here clearly demonstrate how a really terrific academic paper should be developed. On the other hand, upon your demand and for an affordable cost, a pro essay helper with the relevant academic background can put together a top-notch paper example on Kindergarten from scratch.
Bullying Among Children In America: Example Essay By An Expert Writer To Follow
Pedagogical documentation essay example.
Affiliated Institution
Exemplar Essay On Education To Write After
Data Analysis
Comprehensive Classroom Technology Plan Essays Examples
Introduction In this day and age, living within the Internet of Things it would be unwise to ignore the benefits technology can provide in the classroom. When used appropriately it can enhance learning experience for children of all ages; the unprecedented interactivity it provides allows for achieving greater level of involvement in the learning process. This plan makes an attempt to outline some practical uses for technology in kindergarten for improving the learning experience and communication between teacher, parents and children.
Section One: Mission and Vision Statement
Primary Response: Creative Writings Example
Discussion on Hispanic Poetry
Forest School And Risk Taking Research Literature Review Examples
Embracing risk in the Canadian woodlands: Four childrenâs risky play and risk-taking experiences in a Canadian Forest Kindergarten
Forest School Research And Gaps In Studies Literature Review
Essay on raft task card.
This RAFT Card is designed for ____ grade level.
Expertly Crafted Argumentative Essay On The Debate On Common Core Standards
Write by example of this public policy issues within cps essay.
Introduction
Inspiring Essay About Roy Blunt
U.S. Senator, Missouri
2011-Present
Personal Background Martial Status: Married to Abigail Blunt Education: B.A. History, Southwest Baptist University (1970) https://ballotpedia.org/Roy_Blunt M.A. History, Missouri State University (1972) https://ballotpedia.org/Roy_Blunt Previous Experience: Clerk of Greene County, Missouri (1972-1984) https://ballotpedia.org/Roy_Blunt Missouri Secretary of State (1985-1992) https://ballotpedia.org/Roy_Blunt President, Southwest Baptist University (1993-1996) https://ballotpedia.org/Roy_Blunt U.S. House of Representatives (1996-2010) https://ballotpedia.org/Roy_Blunt
Committee Assignments:
Appropriations Committee 2011- present https://ballotpedia.org/Roy_Blunt Commerce. Science, and Transportation Committee 2011-2016 https://ballotpedia.org/Roy_Blunt Rules and Administration Committee 2011- present (became chairman 2015) https://ballotpedia.org/Roy_Blunt Joint Committee on the Library 2015- present https://ballotpedia.org/Roy_Blunt Joint Committee on Printing 2015-present (vice chairman) https://ballotpedia.org/Roy_Blunt Committee on Armed Services 2013-2014 https://ballotpedia.org/Roy_Blunt
Sponsored Legislation
Observation Essays Examples
Classroom Observation Tool
CHS111 Foundations in Early Childhood Education PRINT THIS FORM to use for your classroom observation. Once completed, print this form again and type the information you recorded into the form. The completed, typed form will be submitted in Unit 4.
Observerâs Name: _____Insert your name________________________________________
Time(s): Start: ___9:00am___________ End: _____11:30am___________ CENTER/SCHOOL INFORMATION Program Type (Place âXâ for all that apply): public __ private _Ă_ non-profit __faith-based __ corporate __ other: _____________________
Observed Classroom
Sample Essay On Family Policy
Expertly crafted essay on special education policy case review, essay on âstudyâ, a-level research paper on research project design for free use, expertly written essay on relationship between learning and creativity to follow, good example of research proposal on udl adoption in schools, proper essay example about goal achievements.
Goal Achievement
Ornament And Architecture: Ruskin, Loos, And Sullivan: Essay You Might Want To Emulate
Using play to develop creative thinking skills in preschool/kindergarten: free sample essay to follow.
A Research Proposal First Draft
Smaller Is Better Essay Examples
Proper essay example about misconception diary assignment, research brief: free sample essay to follow, westside community schools research papers example, good essay about growth and development project: observing kindergarten children in a teaching project, good pine crest school: strategic analysis research paper example, good research brief literature review example.
Research Purpose/Introduction
Case Study On Curriculum Plan And Assessment
Curriculum Strategy
Essay On The Difference Between a Parent's and a Child's Childhood
Essay on early childhood.
It is not a surprise to hear people talk of early child hood in the current world and society. The amount of people conversing on childhood development is enormous and greatly increasing since the historical times. Various people have come up with various thoughts and definitions of the same word and several others theorized on what really it involves when someone talks of early child hood. The age at which early childhood starts has sparked a lot of debate and controversies such that up to now it is not clearly visualized when early child hood starts.
The Effect Of The Family On Middle Childhood Development Children From Lower Socioeconomic Backgrounds Essay
The Effect of the Family On Middle Childhood Development: children from lower socio-economic backgrounds.
Essay On The Effective Early Childhood Educator
Research papers on poverty.
Poverty is the lack of basics needs such as health care, clean water, education, nutrition shelter and clothing as a result of inability to afford them. It is also referred to as destitution or absolute poverty. Relative poverty is situation whereby resources are inadequate or less income compared to others within a society or country as compared to worldwide averages. Approximately 1.7 billion people in the world live in poverty. Poverty had become a norm before the industrial revolution.
Long term effects of poverty on children
275 words = 1 page double-spaced

Password recovery email has been sent to [email protected]
Use your new password to log in
You are not register!
By clicking Register, you agree to our Terms of Service and that you have read our Privacy Policy .
Now you can download documents directly to your device!
Check your email! An email with your password has already been sent to you! Now you can download documents directly to your device.
or Use the QR code to Save this Paper to Your Phone
The sample is NOT original!
Short on a deadline?
Don't waste time. Get help with 11% off using code - GETWOWED
No, thanks! I'm fine with missing my deadline
78 Kindergarten Essay Topic Ideas & Examples
đ best kindergarten topic ideas & essay examples, đ simple & easy kindergarten essay titles, đ good essay topics on kindergarten.
- Lev Vygotsky: Educational Implications of Sociocultural Theory The sociocultural theory is more responsive to the learning process of children with special needs since it argues that ideas and concepts are socially mediated and exist in collectives rather than in individuals.
- Vygotskyâs Social-Historical Theory: Towards Facilitating Learning in Kindergarten-Age children It is the object of this paper to demonstrates how Vygotsky’s theory can be used to assist kindergarten students and others with special needs to cope with the learning process It was the perception of […] We will write a custom essay specifically for you by our professional experts 808 writers online Learn More
- Learning To Read With Rubrics. Assisting Kindergarten Learners To Improve Reading Skills The teacher arranges these levels according to the complexity of the content to be taught and evaluated. At the end of this level, the learners should be able to read simple words from a wall […]
- Separate Schooling for the Sexes from Kindergarten to University Level in Saudi Arabia A notable attribute of Saudi Arabia’s education system is that it is highly centralized in nature and the overall supervision is done by the Ministry of Education This ministry is also charged with the training […]
- Comparing Kindergarten Programs in the US to the UK Another aspect of kindergarten curriculum that is identical in the US and in the UK is the fact that it encompasses a lot of play and experienced learning.
- Benefits of Bilingualism among Kindergarten Children The purpose of this report is to show the benefits of learning more than one language among kindergarten children. The purpose of this report is to analyse the benefits of learning two languages among kindergarten […]
- Student Engagement and Student Motivation in a Reading Classroom for the Kindergarten Level The tasks have to be short and able to stimulate the curiosity and creativity of the children This strategy requires that the tasks offered are short enough to allow the children to complete them.
- The Comparison of Montessori Education and a Regular Mathematics Program in Kindergarten Classroom Furthermore, it will compare the strengths and the weaknesses of the Montessori system with a regular mathematics program in order to determine the extent to which it benefits students.
- Education: Bilingual Kindergarten A major problem with bilingualism in kindergartens is that it leads to a lack of mastery in either of the languages.
- Teacher Career: Staff Development in Kindergartens The level of professional development of the teachers at the kindergarten stage plays a key role in the general development of pupils and the quality of education they receive at the end of the day.
- Kindergarten Business Venture: La Trobe University The staff members and students with kids in the university’s kindergarten will be in a position to put more effort into their studies or roles within the institution.
- Full-Day Kindergarten Program: Advantages and Disadvantages The educational stakeholders seek to determine the effect of this program to the parents, teachers, and the pupils undertaking it. In this regard, the full-day kindergarten program provides time for the students to practice the […]
- Kindergarten Classroom Environment Design The design of the classroom environment should reflect the demographic characteristics of learners. The desks of children will be placed right in the center of the room, and at least, four students will sit at […]
- Pre-Kindergarten Education: Program and Resources The challenge that is proposed to be reviewed within the framework of the current letter is the universalization of pre-K education.
- The Superkids Reading Program in the Kindergarten The purpose of this paper is to overview the program components, evaluate it in terms of advantages and disadvantages, and discuss the details of the program implementation in the classroom setting with the focus on […]
- Kindergarten English Language Proficiency Standard 1 It is important to develop an appropriate set of ELP standards for English learners at the kindergarten level in order to create a solid background for basic learning and the linguistic demands that will be […]
- A Pre-Kindergarten Teacher Analysis When speaking about the prestige and relevance of the profession of a pre-kindergarten teacher, it is possible to say that at the national level, this occupation is encouraged, and jobs can be found almost always.
- Reading Aloud in Kindergarten: Lesson Plan The approximate age of learners should be four to five years. The group should consist of approximately 15-20 students.
- Reading Unit Plan for Kindergarten Students The aim of the lesson plan is to teach the students to read and write by the end of the course.
- Learning Second Language in Kindergarten Online Storyland is a section of the Reading Eggs program that allows it to be used with a small group. The program allows teachers to choose from 44 available lesson plans that include student worksheets for […]
- Learning for Kindergarten: Five Senses Objective: By the end of the lesson, the students will be able to define the key five sense and explain, which body organ is responsible for sensing a corresponding signal, as well as define the […]
- Emergent Writing in Abu Dhabi Kindergartens This major variable will be achieved through a sequential implementation of various interactive writing strategies with an emphasis on play-based learning and an active learning approach.
- Kindergarten Inclusion Classroom A kindergarten inclusion classroom needs to meet a number of requirements to be classified as such: it should be aesthetically pleasing, challenging, age-appropriate, and safe for differently-abled children.
- Grouping at the Kindergarten and Secondary Levels Researchers have established that group or cooperative learning improves the academic grades of students and helps in building high self-esteem, good social skills, and enhanced the comprehensive ability of their content and skills of the […]
- Effects of Full-day Kindergarten on Achievement These changes in the American culture and in schooling over the past 2 decades have had their impact on the reputation of full-day, all-day-a-week kindergarten education in a number of communities.
- A Dayâs Outing for Kindergarten Children The client, who is a school, wants the kindergarten children and their children to have a days picnic and outing. The event would help to break the tediousness of regular schoolwork and provide some entertainment […]
- Phonemic Awareness and Word Recognition in Kindergarten Blachman and Ball define phonemic awareness as the awareness that spoken words are made up of individual sounds; the ability to identify the individual phonemes of a word.
- Lead Exposure in Pre-Kindergarten Children Lead poisoning is as a result of accumulation of lead metal in the blood caused by inhalation or ingestion of lead over a period of time.
- Weather and Climate for Kindergarten This lesson sequence is the introductory step towards learning the basic concept of weather and climatic changes in the Earth Science curriculum.
- Services for Students Who Are Transitioning Into Kindergarten The importance of a transfer program aimed at reducing studentsâ exposure to challenges associated with communication and learning.
- Pre-Schooling Before Kindergarten Admission The strategy needs the involvement of the student, and the teacher as the pre-schooling students are young and thus delicate to handle.
- Monitoring Success of Kindergarten Students Thompson and Thompson emphasize the development of creativity as a criterion for engaging instruction. Therefore, to ensure that the curriculum is fun and meets the developmental requirements of children, it is essential to pay attention […]
- The Conflicts in Organizations: Experiences in the Kindergartens The conflicts indicate the latent negative processes, the real existence of disagreements, and the tendency to change them. The nature of the conflict was destructive and contributed to the inability to operate in the workplace.
- Using Technologies in Kindergarten Classrooms Integrating modern technologies into the educational process in kindergartens helps educators transmit knowledge to the children and influence their emotions and perception of the materials.
- Impact of Social Media on Instructional Practices for Kindergarten Teachers General Context of the Problem Despite the increase in the use of social media in teaching, there is still a significant lack of research done on the impact of social media integration in teaching techniques […]
- American Federal Holidays: Essential Knowledge for Kindergarten Their bravery gave a start to the history of our country, as the first European settlers followed Columbus quite soon and established their colonies on American soil. The Fourth of July has become a symbol […]
- Social-Emotional Learning for Kindergarten Education To set adequate goals for social-emotional learning, researchers must quantify and contextualize the behaviors and attitudes of large samples of subjects.
- Counting and Number Line Trainings in Kindergarten: Effects on Arithmetic Performance and Number Sense
- Designing for Human Behaviour: The Elementary Kindergarten
- Factors Affecting the School Readiness of Public Kindergarten Pupils
- Elementary vs. Kindergarten Teacher
- Elementary School Ranges From Kindergarten
- Ecological Instability and Childrenâs Classroom Behavior in Kindergarten
- Pedagogics of the Kindergarten by Friedrich Frobel (1782-1852)
- Computer Science Classes for Students as Early as Kindergarten
- Psychodynamically Oriented Early Prevention for “Children-at-Risk” in Urban Areas
- Elena Developed Chicken Pox When She Was in Kindergarten
- China Pre-school Education (Kindergarten) Industry Overview 2014
- Children and Home Reading Prior to Kindergarten
- Autobiography: High School and Kindergarten Aged Children
- Early Childhood Education and Kindergarten
- Early Kindergarten, Maternal Labor Supply and Children’s Outcomes: Evidence From Italy
- Does Full-Day Kindergarten Matter? Evidence from the First Two Years of Schooling
- Can Universal Pre-kindergarten Programs Improve Population Health and Longevity? Mechanisms, Evidence, and Policy Implications
- Book: Kindergarten and Bibliographic Citation
- Foreign Language Instruction Should Begin in Kindergarten
- Better Beginnings: The State of Early Learning and Kindergarten Readiness in White Center
- Kindergarten Classroom Observation
- Mathematics Education vs. Kindergarten Education
- Kindergarten Teachers Should Have at Least Basic Degree
- How and Why Does Age at Kindergarten Entry Matter
- Math Kindergarten Standard K-4: 2D and 3D Geometric Shapes and Relative Positions in Space
- Lesson Plan For Kindergarten English Learners
- How Does Your Kindergarten Classroom Affect Your Earnings? Evidence From Project Star
- How Non-Native Speaking Children Learn the Chinese Language in Kindergarten
- Observing the Kindergarten Classroom at Pine Meadow Elementary
- Kindergarten Readiness Skills Promotes Academic Achievement
- Full-Day vs. Half-Day Kindergarten
- Investigation into Developed SABIS Curriculum in Kindergarten
- Preschool and Kindergarten Best Practice Curriculum
- Lesson Plans Kindergarten First Grade
- âA Kindergarten Teacher Like Meâ: Social-Emotional Development in Early Childhood Education âA kindergarten teacher like meâ focuses on the relationship between racial and ethnic backgrounds of students and teachers in the social-emotional development of children.
- Pre-Kindergarten Impacts Over Time: An Analysis of KIPP Charter Schools
- Kindergarten Duties and Work Place
- Full Day Kindergarten Programs
- Reading Activity For Kindergarten
- Kindergarten Children and Reading Aloud
- High Quality Pre Kindergarten Classroom Look Like
- Chicago (A-D)
- Chicago (N-B)
IvyPanda. (2023, September 21). 78 Kindergarten Essay Topic Ideas & Examples. https://ivypanda.com/essays/topic/kindergarten-essay-topics/
"78 Kindergarten Essay Topic Ideas & Examples." IvyPanda , 21 Sept. 2023, ivypanda.com/essays/topic/kindergarten-essay-topics/.
IvyPanda . (2023) '78 Kindergarten Essay Topic Ideas & Examples'. 21 September.
IvyPanda . 2023. "78 Kindergarten Essay Topic Ideas & Examples." September 21, 2023. https://ivypanda.com/essays/topic/kindergarten-essay-topics/.
1. IvyPanda . "78 Kindergarten Essay Topic Ideas & Examples." September 21, 2023. https://ivypanda.com/essays/topic/kindergarten-essay-topics/.
Bibliography
IvyPanda . "78 Kindergarten Essay Topic Ideas & Examples." September 21, 2023. https://ivypanda.com/essays/topic/kindergarten-essay-topics/.
- Bilingual Education Essay Ideas
- Invention Topics
- Spanking Ideas
- Literacy Development Titles
- Child Development Research Ideas
- Developmental Psychology Essay Ideas
- Child Welfare Essay Ideas
- Personality Psychology Research Topics
- Childcare Research Topics
- Social Development Essay Topics
- Tolerance Essay Ideas
- Childrenâs Rights Research Ideas
- Pedagogy Topics
- Brain-Based Learning Essay Titles
- Private School Research Ideas

IMAGES
VIDEO
COMMENTS
128 Kindergarten Research Topics & Essay Examples Table of Contents đ Kindergarten Research Papers Examples đ Simple Research Topics about Kindergarten đ Good Kindergarten Essay Topics to Write about đ Best Kindergarten Essay Titles Kindergarten Research Questions Yes, we can! Our experts can deliver a custom Kindergarten paper
Qualitative Study of Kindergarten School Readiness and Personal and Social Development ... Research Question #3: What do kindergarten teachers report they are doing to guide and instruct students who enter kindergarten without the necessary personal and social skills to
Play-Based Early Childhood Classrooms and the Effect on Pre-Kindergarten Social and Academic Achievement A Graduate Research Paper Submitted to the Division of Early Childhood Education Department of Curriculum and Instruction In Partial Fulfillment Of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in Education UNIVERSITY OF NORTHERN IOWA By Amy...
kindergarten) can be described as a combination of the following skills: social skills, language, and literacy skills, ability to follow directions and engage, possess self-control, and exhibit common courtesy (Hustedt, Buell, Hallam, & Pinder, 2017; Samiei, Bush, Sell,
A PLAY-BASED RESEARCH STUDY: CHILDREN AT PLAY ACQUIRING LITERACY SKILLS SOCIALLY AND THROUGH SELF-DISCOVERY 2014 Valerie Lee, Ph. D. Master of Arts in Reading The purposes of this qualitative research were to observe a group of kindergarten students during play exploration and determine how kindergarten students acquire
are occupied on doing loads of paper works and other school-related tasks that restrict them in ... Kindergarten population of approximately 20,000 out of about 1,800,000 total populations of ... (Bowen, 2009) and an important basis in qualitative research similar to interviews (Puvenesvary et al., 2008). It scrutinized and interpreted the data ...
This paper provides an example of a research about children's view of decision-making in a kindergarten, exemplifying the possibilities of research with children. Discover the world's research ...
The study's title, "Invented Spelling in Kindergarten as a Predictor of Reading and Spelling in Grade 1: A New Pathway to Literacy, or Just the Same Road, Less Known?" (Ouellette and Sénéchal,...
framework of collaborative action research. Sample: The study was conducted by a university researcher in collaboration with 4 kindergarten teachers and 4 parent representatives from 2 kindergarten classes in North-Eastern Greece. 35 children and their parents participated in the project.
â Examples: self and peer assessment, Citations (37) References (39) ... Kindergarten teachers face challenges in designing assessment strategies that encompass manifold functions of the...
This video series will teach you how to write a research paper or report. Each episode leads you through each step of the writing process. In this video, you...
The sample size was 299 teachers drawn from both public and private kindergarten schools in the Kumasi ... (Paper) ISSN 2222-288X (Online) Vol.7, No.36, 2016 ... âą Research question: What are kindergarten teachers' self-efficacy beliefs about their classroom
In order to assist education majors researching topics pertaining to kindergarten classrooms, Paper Masters has outlined a sample research study on comparing a Public Montessori Kindergarten classroom to a traditional Kindergarten classroom.
Research Report Writing Unit - Kindergarten, 1st Grade Research Study Template. Teach your kindergarten or first grade students the report writing process with this NO PREP writing package! This Report Writing Unit includes:âąTeacher Lesson PlanâąK-W-L ChartâąFacts 4 Square Graphic OrganizerâąReport Cover PageâąTable of Contentsâą4 pages ...
The American Educational Research Association (AERA), founded in 1916, is concerned with improving the educational process by encouraging scholarly inquiry related to education and evaluation and by promoting the dissemination and practical application of research results. AERA is the most prominent international professional organization, with the primary goal of advancing educational ...
Ancient Civilizations. Last but certainly not least is Ancient Civilizations! This is a really good research topic for kids because there are so many ancient civilizations to choose from. Some examples of ancient civilizations are the Greeks, Incas, Aztecs, Egyptians, Maya, Persian, Roman, and Chinese.
View sample early childhood education research paper. Browse research paper examples for more inspiration. If you need a psychology research paper written according to all the academic standards, you can always turn to our experienced writers for help. This is how your paper can get an A! Feel free to contact our writing service for professional assistance.
Get your free examples of research papers and essays on Kindergarten here. Only the A-papers by top-of-the-class students. Learn from the best! ... The observational research design was used to address these hypotheses. A purposive sample of 50 children (30 boys and 20 girls) were recruited for the study and data gathered by observation. From ...
Abstract Introduction This research examined the classification accuracy of the Quick Interactive Language Screener (QUILS) for identifying preschool-aged children (3;0 to 6;9) with developmental language disorder (DLD). We present data from two independent samples that varied in prevalence and diagnostic reference standard.
research on special programs for handicapped children. Twenty-eight research documents were reviewed in preparation for this report. Eighteen were studies, eight were reviews, and two reported the results of both a study and a review effort. Seventeen reported the results of research conducted with preschool children, six concerned
The approximate age of learners should be four to five years. The group should consist of approximately 15-20 students. Reading Unit Plan for Kindergarten Students. The aim of the lesson plan is to teach the students to read and write by the end of the course. Learning Second Language in Kindergarten Online.
This paper seeks to address the question on whether or not kindergarten students learn during free reading time employing action research as a means of research. Using action research as a methodology, significant changes have been implemented by reading teachers across grades. Included in these studies have been voluntary reading studies.
Sample Research Paper For Kindergarten, Project Assistant Cover Letter Examples, Best Annotated Bibliography Editor Service Online, Formulate A Business Plan For A Particular Project, Free Essay On Problem Solving, Dissertation Sur La Litterature General, 3. Describe a moment in your life that began with a significant experience and ended with ...